Skip to main content

The independence of deficits in position sense and visually guided reaching following stroke

Abstract

Background

Several studies have found correlations between proprioception and visuomotor function during stroke recovery, however two more recent studies have found no correlation. Unfortunately, most of the studies to date have been conducted with clinical assessments of sensation that are observer-based and have poor reliability. We have recently developed new tests to assess position sense and motor function using robotic technology. The present study was conducted to reassess the relationship between position sense and upper limb movement following stroke.

Methods

We assessed position sense and motor performance of 100 inpatient stroke rehabilitation subjects and 231 non-disabled controls. All subjects completed quantitative assessments of position sense (arm-position matching task) and motor performance (visually-guided reaching task) using the KINARM robotic device. Subjects also completed clinical assessments including handedness, vision, Purdue Pegboard, Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment-Impairment Inventory and Functional Independence Measure (FIM). Neuroimaging documented lesion localization. Fisher’s exact probability tests were used to determine the relationship between performances on the arm-position matching and visually-guided reaching task. Pearson’s correlations were conducted to determine the relationship between robotically measured parameters and clinical assessments.

Results

Performance by individual subjects on the matching and reaching tasks was statistically independent (Fisher’s test, P<0.01). However, performance on the matching and reaching tasks both exhibited relationships with abilities in daily activities as measured by the FIM. Performance on the reaching task also displayed strong relationships with other clinical measures of motor impairment.

Conclusions

Our data support the concept that position sense deficits are functionally relevant and point to the importance of assessing proprioceptive and motor impairments independently when planning treatment strategies.

Background

Sensory deficits are particularly common following stroke, occurring in up to 70% of patients [1]. Many studies have indicated a connection between impaired sensation and functional recovery of the upper extremity. In particular, proprioceptive deficits have been shown to negatively impact safety, postural stability and motor function [2]. Impaired proprioception has also been shown to have prognostic significance in self-care, likelihood of discharge home and length of hospital stay [37].

Although some reports have indicated that position sense, a sub-component of proprioception [8], strongly correlates with motor recovery of the hemiplegic arm after stroke [913], two studies have failed to support this relationship [14, 15]. One of the studies [14] that failed to demonstrate a relationship used the Thumb Localizer Test [16] of proprioception while the other relied on a simple clinician administered two point rating scale (impaired vs. normal) [15]. These and similar clinical assessments of proprioception have been shown to have poor inter- and intra-rater reliability and tend to use coarse ordinal scales [17, 18]. Recent technological advances have the potential to create better measurements of proprioception and motor function in patients with stroke [1923]. We have developed reliable robotic assessments of position sense [24] and visuomotor function [25] in patients with stroke. This has created the opportunity to re-evaluate the relationship between proprioceptive, motor and functional deficits.

A better understanding of the relationship between proprioception and motor deficits is clinically important because it can guide rehabilitation. Traditionally, stroke rehabilitation has placed a significant focus on restoring motor function with little emphasis on recovery of proprioception. Clinically, we have observed patients with impaired proprioception who have sub-optimal functional recovery despite treatment with existing evidence-based motor rehabilitation treatments. A recent review of rehabilitation for sensory loss following stroke [26] examined a number of small studies, some of which demonstrate potential for clinical use. Many of these studies used motor outcome measures to evaluate passive sensory retraining treatments [2734]. Given sensory and motor recovery may not be as strongly correlated as once thought, this strategy may be inappropriate.

A fundamental part of developing a treatment for stroke is to first evaluate the extent and nature of the stroke-related deficits. It is thus desirable to have appropriate tools for assessment of sensory impairments that are independent of motor assessment tools in order to interpret the effects of rehabilitation strategies on the sensory system. At the level of the individual patient, a more in-depth understanding of the patient’s exact impairments creates the ability to develop targeted and personalized rehabilitation strategies. For instance, two patients may present with the inability to perform a particular activity of daily living. Yet the reason for this may be related to impairment in sensation, motor function or both. Our anecdotal experience suggests that both these patients would receive the same “one size fits most” treatment approach in our centres. Better assessment may allow us to target treatment at one system versus the other, or both if necessary. The primary objective of the present study was to examine the relationship between upper extremity position sense deficits and visuomotor deficits following stroke. We used a robotic assessment of position sense and made comparisons to motor performance on a robotic visually-guided reaching task. Our secondary objective was to make comparisons of performance on the robotically administered assessments to more traditional clinical outcome tools: the Purdue Pegboard (PPB), the Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment - Impairment Inventory (CMSA) of the arm and hand, and the FIM. Given that many groups have reported links between proprioception and motor outcomes, one might expect that subjects with stroke with impaired position sense should demonstrate impairments on many of these measures.

Methods

Subjects

Subjects with stroke were recruited from the inpatient stroke rehabilitation ward at Providence Care, St. Mary’s of the Lake Hospital site in Kingston, Ontario, Canada and the Foothills Medical Centre in Calgary, Alberta, Canada. Non-disabled control subjects were recruited from the communities of Calgary, Alberta, Canada or Kingston, Ontario, Canada. The ethics review boards of Queen’s University, Providence Care, and the University of Calgary approved the study. All subjects provided informed consent prior to participating in the study.

Subjects were included in the study if they were 18 years of age or older and were able to understand the instructions required to complete the assessments. Subjects were excluded from the study if they had ongoing musculoskeletal problems of the upper extremity or history of neurological disorders other than stroke. Subjects with stroke were also excluded if hemispatial neglect was confirmed by the conventional subset of the Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT).

Stroke lesion locations were documented using neuroimaging (Computed Tomography or Magnetic Resonance Imaging). Within the rest of this text, rather than referencing subjects with stroke by the side of their lesion, we refer to the clinically most affected side of their body.

Robotic assessment

Robotic device

The robotic assessment was performed using the KINARM exoskeleton robotic device (BKIN Technologies Ltd., Kingston, Ontario) [35]. Subjects sat in a modified wheelchair base with each arm supported in the horizontal plane by the robotic exoskeleton. The device permits arm movements in the horizontal plane, monitors shoulder and elbow motion and can apply mechanical loads at the shoulder and/or elbow. Subjects were allowed free head movement and viewed an augmented reality system that could display visual targets in the same plane of the arms. Details of the tasks and analyses used in this study have been previously described [24, 25].

Assessment of position sense: Arm-position matching task

With the arms and hands occluded from vision, the robot passively moved one hand (passive arm) to one of nine spatial locations separated by 10 cm (Figure 1A,B). When the robot completed the passive movement, subjects actively moved their opposite hand (active arm) in an attempt to mirror match the passive hand position. Subjects were permitted as much time as necessary to match the passive hand location before cuing the experimenter to trigger the next trial. Each subject completed 6 randomized blocks for a total of 54 trials. For subjects with stroke, the robot passively moved the arm on their affected side. Controls were tested on both arms.

Figure 1
figure 1

Exemplar control subject data . A) Arm-position matching task as seen from above. On the left, the positions of the robotically moved hand are filled symbols; positions of the actively moved hand are open symbols. Ellipses represent 1 standard deviation. B) Passive (robotically moved) hand positions have been mirrored onto those of the active hand for visualization purposes. C) Individual hand paths for movements to each of the eight targets in the visually guided reaching task. D) Velocity profiles for hand paths from the centre to the rightward target.

Three parameters quantified position sense: 1) trial-to-trial Variability of the active hand (Varxy), 2) Contraction/Expansion of the spatial area enclosed by the active hand relative to the spatial area enclosed by the passive hand (Areaxy), and 3) Systematic Shifts between the passive and active hands (Shiftxy). The formulae for these parameters have been previously described and provide reliable, 2-dimensional quantification of common deficits in position sense, and may describe different physiologic/functional attributes of limb position sense [24]. For this study, Contraction/Expansion was normalized by control performance to create positive scores for both contraction and expansion. Values between 0 and 1 indicated performance within the 95% confidence bounds of non-disabled controls, whereas values greater than 1 indicated abnormal contraction or expansion.

Assessment of motor performance: visually guided reaching task

With full vision of the arms and hands, subjects were instructed to reach “as quickly and accurately as possible” from a central target (near the centre of the arm’s workspace) to one of eight peripheral targets located 10 cm away (Figure 1C,D). The robot provided no active resistance or assistance for the reaching movements. Each trial began with the subjects holding their index finger tip at the central target for 1250–1750 ms before the peripheral target was illuminated thereby signaling to initiate their reaching movement. Subjects were provided 3000 ms to complete each reach. Each subject completed 8 randomized blocks. Two catch trials, in which no peripheral target was displayed, were included in each block. In total, each subject completed 80 trials with each arm.

We report five movement parameters, one from each of five attributes of sensorimotor control: a) postural control, b) reaction time, c) initial movement d) corrective movements, and e) total movement metrics. The algorithms for determining all parameters have been previously described in detail [25]. Postural control was characterized by postural hand speed (PS) for the 500ms prior to peripheral target illumination. Reaction time (RT) was the time from peripheral target onset until movement onset. The initial phases of movement from movement onset to the first speed minimum were quantified using initial movement direction error (IDE). IDE is the angular deviation between a straight line from the central to peripheral target and the actual path taken in the initial phase of movement (first minima of hand speed after movement onset). Corrective movements after their initial response were quantified by recording the number of speed peaks per movement (NSP). The total movement metric used was total movement time (MT). These parameters provided the best combination of high reliability and sensitivity (ie. the ability to detect the presence of an impairment relative to normal behaviour) for each attribute [25].

Clinical assessment

The clinical assessment consisted of a broad range of sensory, motor, and functional evaluations. These measures were chosen as the majority of them are used in day-to-day practice in our stroke rehabilitation centres.

Handedness was documented with the Modified Edinburgh Handedness inventory [36]. Subjects were considered Right handed (R) if they scored >50, Left handed (L) if they scored <−50 and Mixed handedness (M) if they scored >−50 and <50.

Position sense was assessed clinically with the Thumb Localizer Test (TLT) [16], chosen because of its previous use quantifying whole-limb position sense in several studies of subjects with stroke [14, 3743]. As described by Hirayama [16], the subject, with eyes closed, extends one thumb and the examiner moves that arm to a position in front of the subject at or above eye level, lateral to the midline. The subject is then asked to pinch the extended thumb with the opposite thumb and forefinger. Subjects are scored 0 (accurately does the task) to 3 (subject is unable to find his or her thumb and does not climb up the affected arm to locate it). Subjects with stroke localized the thumb on their affected arm whereas controls localized the thumb on their dominant arm.

Spasticity was assessed using the Modified Ashworth Scale [44]. In this assessment, the examiner moves the subject’s limb through a range of motion and evaluates the resistance to passive movement. Scores range from 0 (no increase in muscle tone) to 4 (affected part is rigid in flexion or extension). In the present manuscript we report the MAS for the elbow flexors.

Motor impairment was assessed using the Purdue Pegboard (PPB) (LaFayette Instrument Co., LaFayette, IN, USA) [45]. In this assessment the subject attempts to place as many pegs as possible in a board in a 30 second time window with one hand. While this is traditionally touted as a test of fine motor skills, the subject is required to use the proximal upper extremity to get the hand to the correct position to retrieve and insert each peg.

The Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment - Impairment Inventory (CMSA) [46] was performed to stage the arm and hand. This tool relies on the concept of stages of motor recovery, first introduced by Twitchell [1], whereby a patient is thought to progress through a sequence of stages from flaccid paralysis (Stage 1) to normal movement (Stage 7) as they recover from a stroke. Individuals are scored based on their ability to perform a set of predetermined movements in front of an examiner. The authors of the tool indicate that the “Stages of Motor Recovery” serve as a measurement for the amount of neurological impairment.

Functional abilities were documented with the Functional Independence Measure (FIM) [47]. This 18 item scale scores individuals across a number of items. There are 13 items considered to be motor tasks (eating, grooming, bathing, upper and lower body dressing, toileting, bladder and bowel management, bed to chair transfers, toilet and shower transfers, locomotion and the ability to do stairs). There are 5 items considered to be cognitive tasks (cognitive comprehension, expression, social interaction, problem solving and memory). Each item is ranked on a 7-point ordinal scale that ranges from 1 (total dependence) to 7 (complete independence). In the present manuscript we present the total FIM score (measured out of 126) and the FIM self-care subscore (FIMsc – measured out of 42, items include: eating, grooming, bathing, dressing above the waist, dressing below the waist and toileting).

Visual acuity was assessed with a Snellen eye chart and visual fields were examined using confrontation [48]. Visuospatial attention was assessed using the conventional subset of the Behavioural Inattention Task (BIT) [49]. The BIT is a standardized assessment for visuospatial inattention (also known as hemispatial neglect) with a number of pencil and paper tests including line, letter and star cancellation, line bisection, figure copying and drawing.

Stroke lesion locations were recorded by one of the study physicians with experience in neuroimaging (S.P.D). Clinical computed tomography (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans and the clinical reports generated by a neuroradiologist were evaluated. From this a standard form was used to document the side of the lesion, the vascular territory (eg. right middle cerebral artery), and brain region (egs. cortical, subcortical, brainstem, cerebellum).

Non-disabled controls completed the same assessments as subjects with stroke with a few exceptions. No control subject performed the FIM or BIT. A subset of non-disabled controls performed the TLT, MAS and CMSA to obtain some idea about the amount of variability amongst controls in these clinical measures.

A trained study physician or a physiotherapist with expertise in stroke rehabilitation performed all clinical assessments.

Statistical comparisons

Statistical Analyses were performed using MATLAB (Mathworks, Inc., Massachusetts, USA). Subjects with stroke were flagged for impairments on robotic parameters when their performance fell outside a parameter’s normative reference range, defined as the 95% confidence boundaries of control performance taking age [5053], sex [5456], and test-arm (dominant/non-dominant) [5762] into consideration. Our primary analysis was concerned with the performance of the most affected arm in subjects with stroke. To establish each parameter’s normative reference range, control values were tested for normality (Lillifors test, P< 0.01) and transformed with a log, square root, or inverse transform to obtain normality when necessary. Regression analyses were then used to model the effects of age, gender and test-arm when creating normative reference ranges for each stroke subject.

Impairments on the parameters from each task were used to classify whether a subject had performed “normally” or “abnormally” on either the arm position matching task, the visually-guided reaching task, both or neither. Fisher’s exact probability tests and Pearson’s correlation tests were used to quantify relationships between robotic and clinical measures. The p value of significance on these tests was set at p < 0.01. Bonferroni corrections were applied to tests with multiple comparisons.

Results

Subject pool

Data were collected from 100 subjects with stroke and 231 non-disabled controls. Demographic and clinical features of the groups are provided in Table 1. Left- and right-affected stroke groups were similar with respect to age, gender, handedness and time since stroke. Spasticity of the affected arm, as clinically measured by the MAS, was evident in about half the subjects with stroke (n = 48), but was typically mild or moderate in severity (1, 1+, or 2, n = 46). FIM scores were similar between left- and right-affected stroke groups (Wilcoxon rank sum test, P = 0.07). While most FIM scores fell within the mild range for stroke severity (FIM >= 80, n = 78), many were found within the moderate and severe ranges (FIM < 80, n = 22). Eleven left- and eight right-affected subjects with stroke demonstrated evidence of hemianopsia during confrontation testing. No subjects with evidence of hemispatial neglect (BIT < 130) were included in the present study.

Table 1 Subject characteristics

Robotic data

Individual subject exemplars

Figure 1A,B shows arm position matching results of a healthy 67 year old female control subject. The robot passively moved the left arm and the subject actively moved the right arm to the mirror location in space. As with most non-disabled controls, variability about each target is relatively small (< 5 cm) and there is little spatial contraction/expansion or systematic shift. Figure 1C,D depicts reaching performance of the same subject. The modest curvatures in the hand-paths (Figure 1C) and moderate variability in the velocity profiles (Figure 1D) are typical for a non-disabled control subject of this age.

Table 2 provides the actual values for the robotic parameters for the 67 year-old right handed female depicted in Figure 1. The robotic parameters that demonstrated a significant effect of age, sex or test-arm in the non-disabled controls are also identified with an X in Table 2. Further, the median, interquartile range and normative reference range (parameter values at the limit of what is considered to be within the control range) are presented for both a 27 year-old and a 67 year-old right-handed female for comparison purposes. Notice the specific differences in the normative reference ranges between the two.

Table 2 Significant effects of age, sex, and test-arm for each parameter with the corresponding normative statistics at two distinct ages, 27 and 67 years old

Figure 2 illustrates the performance of subjects with stroke in the matching and reaching tasks. These exemplar subjects illustrate that statistically established deficits in the matching task are not necessarily coupled with significant deficits in the reaching task, and vice versa.

Figure 2
figure 2

Exemplar data from subjects with stroke . Data are presented in the left column for position matching, and the middle and right hand columns for visually guided reaching movements. A &B) Two subjects with stroke who performed within normal limits on both tasks. C &D) Two subjects with stroke who performed outside the normal range on the matching task, but were normal on the reaching task. E &F) Two subjects with stroke who performed normally on the matching task, but were abnormal on the reaching task. G &H) Two subjects with stroke who performed abnormally on both tasks.

Figure 2A,B displays two exemplar subjects with stroke whose performance was not statistically different from non-disabled controls. Figure 2A shows a right-affected subject with an ischemic stroke of the left pontine artery 28 days prior to robotic testing. A right-affected subject who experienced a hemorrhagic stroke of the left basilar artery 29 days before robotic assessment is illustrated in Figure 2B. Despite their different stroke types, their reaching and matching performance is qualitatively and quantitatively similar to controls.

Other subjects with stroke performed abnormally on the matching task but normally on the reaching task (Figure 2C,D). Figure 2C depicts a right-affected subject with an ischemic stroke of the left anterior cerebral artery 9 days prior to robotic testing. This subject demonstrates a significant systematic shift in the matching task, yet is unimpaired on the reaching task. Figure 2D depicts data from a left-affected subject who had an ischemic stroke of the right middle cerebral artery (MCA) 59 days before robotic examination. This subject exhibits significantly increased variability in the matching task, but normal reaching behaviour with the affected left arm.

Another group of subjects with stroke performed normally on the matching task, but abnormally on the reaching task (Figure 2E,F). Figure 2E displays a left-affected subject with an ischemic stroke of the left pontine artery 24 days before robotic testing. Figure 2F illustrates a left-affected subject with an ischemic stroke of the right MCA 24 days prior to robotic assessment. These subjects perform qualitatively and quantitatively similar to controls in the matching task. However, there are clear abnormalities in the hand paths (middle) and multiple peaks in the velocity profile (right) consistent with deficits in reaching.

A final group of subjects with stroke performed abnormally on both robotic tasks. Figure 2G depicts this pattern in a subject with an ischemic left MCA stroke 15 days prior to robotic examination. Figure 2H shows a subject with an ischemic right PCA stroke 19 days prior to robotic assessment.

Stroke group data

Our key observation around our primary objective was that deficits in arm-position matching were largely independent of deficits in visually-guided reaching across the group studied. Table 3 displays the number of subjects found in each category (deficits in matching, reaching, neither or both) and these numbers are not significantly different from those predicted if the two behaviours were completely independent of each other (Fisher’s exact probability test, P = 0.12).

Table 3 Relationship between the matching and reaching tasks and clinical measures

A more detailed demonstration of this independence is seen by examining the categorical relationships (Fisher’s exact probability tests) and correlation coefficients (Pearson’s correlations) of individual parameters within and between the tasks (Table 4). In the matching task, Fisher’s tests demonstrated significant categorical relationships between Variability, Contraction/Expansion, and Systematic Shifts (All P < 0.0014). Additionally, Pearson’s correlations revealed significant relationships between Variability and Shift (P < 0.0014) with a correlation of 0.36. Figure 3A illustrates the relationship between Variability and Systematic Shifts. Far more subjects failed (diamonds) or passed (circles) on both parameters than expected by random chance, whereas far fewer subjects had deficits in only one of the two parameters (squares, triangles).

Table 4 Relationships between matching and reaching measures
Figure 3
figure 3

A) Scatter plot of matching parameters Variability (Var xy ) vs Systematic Shift (Shift xy ). Grey shading represents normal control performance on each parameter. Tick marks indicate regions where normal performance can vary due to each subject’s age, gender, and/or active arm. Circles: subjects with stroke who perform normally on both parameters. Diamonds: subjects with stroke who perform abnormally on both parameters. Squares: subjects with stroke who perform abnormally on Varxy only. Triangles: subjects with stroke who perform abnormally on Shiftxy only. B) Scatter plot of reaching parameters Movement Time (MT) versus Initial Direction Error (IDE). Shading and symbols similar to above. C) Scatter plot of reaching parameter MT versus position matching parameter Varxy. Shading and symbols similar to above.

Within the reaching task, a similar trend was observed across parameters except for relationships with PS (Table 4). The relationship between the parameters MT and IDE is shown in Figure 3B. Similar to the matching task, more subjects exhibited deficits (diamonds) or were normal (circles) on both parameters and fewer subjects showed deficits in only one of the two parameters (squares, triangles) than would be expected by chance alone (Fisher’s test, P < 0.0014). Additionally, there was a significant Pearson’s correlation between these two parameters (r = 0.67, P < 0.0014). Except for the weak relationship between RT and NSP, all other relationships between RT, MT, IDE, and NSP showed statistical categorical results (Fisher’s tests, P < 0.0014).

In contrast to the relationships within each task, there was substantively less statistical interactions between the matching and reaching tasks (Table 4). The relationship between Variability (matching) and Movement Time (reaching) is illustrated in Figure 3C. The number of subjects identified as performing abnormally on both parameters, one only or neither was essentially randomly distributed (Fisher’s test, P = 0.047) and the Pearson’s correlation was not significant (r = 0.19, P > 0.0014). In fact, only one pair of reaching and matching parameters, Shiftxy and PS, exhibited a significant categorical relationship (Fisher’s test, P < 0.0014) and none of the matching and reaching parameters demonstrated a significant Pearson’s correlation (all P > 0.0014).

Effect of visuomotor impairment of the ipsilesional arm

A small number of subjects with stroke in the present study (n = 9) demonstrated impairment in the ipsilesional (less affected) arm that caused them to have difficulty getting to visually guided reaching targets in more than 5% of trials. As part of a secondary analysis, we removed these individuals from the data set and again compared the relationship between the visually guided reaching task and the position matching task. We observed that deficits in arm position matching were still largely independent of those in visually guided reaching (Fisher’s test, P = 0.22).

Relationship between robotic assessment and clinical measures of impairment and disability

Although there were minimal interactions between the matching and reaching tasks, both tasks often correlated with clinical measures of impairment and disability (Table 5). All three matching parameters had a significant categorical relationship with FIM (Fisher’s test, P < 0.0013) while Variability and Systematic Shifts also exhibited significant Pearson’s correlations (P < 0.0013). Except for Reaction Time, the reaching parameters also showed either a significant categorical relationship (PS, IDE: Fisher’s test, P < 0.0013) and/or Pearson’s correlation (IDE, NSP, MT: P < 0.0013) with the FIM. Similarly, some reaching parameters also displayed significant categorical relationships with the Purdue Pegboard (RT: Fisher’s test, P < 0.0013) and Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment - Impairment Inventory (RT, MT: Fisher’s tests, P < 0.0013). Further, most reaching parameters displayed Pearson’s correlations (RT, IDE, NSP, MT: P < 0.0013) with the Purdue Pegboard. The Thumb Localizing Test had significant categorical relationships with Varxy and Shiftxy (Fisher’s test, P < 0.0013). However, it also showed a significant categorical relationship with NSP (Fisher’s test, P < 0.0013).

Table 5 Relationships between individual robotic measures and clinical measures

Relationship between lesion location and performance on robotic measures

Taking into account the side, vascular territory and the cortical or sub-cortical nature of the stroke lesions, we assessed the lesions to determine whether certain locations might be more commonly associated with deficits in the two tasks. A large cohort of our subjects with stroke had cortical or sub-cortical lesions of the MCA territory (29 right and 31 left). Subjects with identified deficits for reach only, match only and both tasks were 4, 3 and 21 for right MCA lesions and 13, 0 and 17 for left MCA respectively. More subjects displayed deficits in reach only with left MCA lesions compared to right MCA lesions, the reverse was true for match only deficits. However, these differences did not reach statistical difference (Fisher’s test, p = 0.98). As well, the distribution of deficits across the two tasks for subjects with lesions in the MCA territory were also not different than the distributions observed for the remaining subjects with lesions elsewhere in the brain (Fisher’s test, p = 0.11).

Discussion

Our findings point to the fact that position sense, as assessed by the arm-position matching task, was largely independent of deficits in motor performance, as assessed by the visually-guided reaching task. Importantly, deficits in position sense were significantly correlated with the FIM. The FIM is a rehabilitation instrument that measures functional abilities [47] and is widely used by stroke rehabilitation clinicians in North America. Thus, we included it in the present study. However, the FIM is, perhaps, not the most sensitive instrument for upper-limb function, as many domains do not include upper limb function. Further, the FIM collapses across both motor and cognitive function.

Despite the above caveats, our findings still imply that impaired position sense has an impact on daily activities, something that mirrors our clinical experience. This point is important because many traditional rehabilitation strategies [63] focus heavily on remediating or compensating for motor deficits. In contrast, sensory deficits typically receive little attention and yet a substantial proportion of subjects display impairments in proprioception. This raises the following question: if proprioceptive deficits are so common and interfere with function, why do clinicians often choose to ignore them in rehabilitation? Some may put forward that a substantial amount of evidence exists for rehabilitating motor function after stroke while there is little evidence for techniques aimed at improving sensory function. Perhaps the problem and the circularity of this argument come back to assessment. It is difficult, if not impossible, to demonstrate efficacy in a clinical study without an appropriate assessment tool. Tests of proprioceptive function have typically been unreliable [17]. In clinical practice, we have seen patients with large deficits in position sense and clinicians who incorrectly attributed the patient’s difficulty to issues with muscle tone or visual spatial processing. The present results suggest that rehabilitation therapy should address proprioceptive dysfunction and also explore new treatments for these deficits [18, 2224].

In addition to the FIM, we also found significant relationships between reaching performance and other clinical measures of movement. Our findings parallel those by Bosecker et al. [21] and add to the validity of using robotics for post-stroke assessment. The robotic technique has many strengths including objectivity, reliability and the fact it does not rely on an ordinal scale. The present study demonstrated greater sensitivity of the robotic technique for detecting motor impairments than a more traditional clinical assessment, the CMSA. This is highlighted by the 23 subjects with stroke who had normal CMSA scores but detectable abnormalities in the robotic reaching task.

More generally, robotic assessment may have many benefits and potential uses [19]. Monitoring the effectiveness of an intervention could be relatively easy, especially in situations where typical clinical scales suffer from poor sensitivity to change and reliability. Robotic assessments may provide insight into potential areas for targeted therapeutics, as the assessments can be designed to more specifically and precisely interrogate deficits than is possible with the naked eye. It is not uncommon in our clinical practice to encounter individuals after stroke that complain of subtle impairments that are not well elucidated by existing clinical measures. Robotic assessments may be helpful in objective identification of these impairments and aid in justifying further treatment. Importantly, for studying interventions, robotic testing has the potential to rapidly and reliably identify individuals with and without certain deficits, thus allowing the possibility of selection of a more homogenous study population.

We included the Thumb Localizer Test in the present study as it was the closest clinical standard for assessing whole upper extremity position sense reported in the literature. Our robotic measure of position sense exhibited a significant relationship with the TLT [16], but the two tests did not always identify abnormalities in the same subjects. Admittedly, there are differences in the conduct of the two tests. Unlike the arm-position matching task, subjects must cross the midline in the TLT. Some caution must be taken in interpreting the present results as versions of the TLT with a binary outcome scale have been shown to have poor reliability [17].

The data in this paper are from a heterogeneous group of sub-acute stroke rehabilitation patients. Inclusion criteria were specifically selected so that study subjects would appropriately represent individuals from sub-acute in-patient stroke rehabilitation. These criteria were chosen to aid in the generalizability of the results. We acknowledge our analysis of the imaging data was relatively simplistic at the present time, focusing only on the vascular territory involved in the stroke. Future studies with more detailed anatomic and volumetric analyses of lesions may be helpful in shedding light on the neuroanatomic correlates of impairments in position sense and visually guided reaching, but are beyond the scope of the present paper.

In the present study we collapsed our measures across target locations in both the position matching task and visually guided reaching tasks. A few recent reports in healthy individuals have indicated that targets located closer to the body are more clearly represented in the brain [64, 65]. Determining what impact this may or may not have on the relationship between the two tasks was also considered beyond the scope of the present manuscript, but does highlight the potential for future study.

The robotic device used in the present study does not allow for sensorimotor assessment of the hand. However, we showed significant relationships between the robotic measures and the FIM as well as the FIM self-care subscore implying the potential importance of proximal limb impairments in overall function. This is consistent with a recent study demonstrating that the initial level of impairment in the proximal upper extremity is predictive of overall upper limb recovery after stroke [66].

The present robotic device functions in the horizontal plane. This position has been recommended for performing assessments in the upper limb as the device supports the limb against gravity [67]. Thus, patients with weakness following stroke are able to engage in assessments that would not be possible otherwise. This is highly advantageous in the clinical setting where early assessment may hold the key to important prognostic information. While three-dimensional assessment of position sense with gravitational support may be possible in more costly and complicated devices, it is not clear that it would add substantively to our ability to detect position sense impairments. This is because the primary peripheral detector for position sense is thought to be the muscle spindle [68, 69] and most muscles are stretched in both horizontal and vertical movements. This fact, and our clinical experience with the current robotic technique lead us to believe the present results are generalizable to movements in three dimensions.

Due to the very nature of the position sense task, the robot passively moved the subject’s relaxed arm, thus depriving subjects of efference copy signals that are present in neurologically intact individuals during active movement. One could argue that position sense in subjects with stroke should improve if they have access to this information. However we did not specifically evaluate this and would require a different experimental paradigm aimed at determining the relative contributions of afferent information and efference copy.

As the present study featured prospective data collected at a single time point following stroke, it is difficult to predict whether the relationships we observed will be preserved over the entire trajectory of stroke recovery. Investigations with a longitudinal study would be able to quantify the nature of the relationship of proprioception and motor function over time and improve our understanding of the trajectory of proprioceptive recovery following stroke.

Despite the limitations listed above, the separation of deficits of position sense and those in reaching movements is somewhat surprising given the importance of proprioceptive feedback in voluntary motor control [70]. At one level, the independence of deficits on the matching and reaching task might have resulted from the fact that the matching task is a bimanual activity that requires more interhemispheric transfer than the unimanual reaching task. Further, the matching task requires an additional spatial transformation to “flip” the coordinates of the target to the opposite side of the workspace for matching. Thus, the separation between the two tasks may be a reflection of the more complex spatial demands of the matching task versus the reaching task.

However, another possibility is the suggestion of a potential distinction between using limb afferents for perception (position sense) versus using them for action (feedback control), akin to the distinction between the use of vision for perception and action [71]. Goodale and Milner [71] proposed two different streams for processing visual information, one stream responsible for processing information for the purpose of perception and a second stream responsible for processing visual information needed to undertake action. Studies in vision have largely supported this conceptual framework [72, 73]. Although our data is suggestive of a similar separation in the use of limb afferents for perception versus for action, the reaching task involved the use of vision that may have compensated for impairments in the use of limb afferent information. Thus, further refinement of the motor task to remove the use of vision is necessary to verify if there is a distinction between perception and action for limb afferents.

Conclusions

In the present study group, performance on the position sense task was independent of that on the visually guided reaching task. It also appears that position sense deficits impact performance of activities of daily living. This study points to the importance of assessing proprioceptive and motor impairments independently when planning treatment strategies or measuring the outcomes of interventions.

Authors’ information

SPD is a practicing Physiatrist who specializes in stroke rehabilitation. He routinely sees patients with sensory deficits following stroke.

Funding

This work was supported by a CIHR operating grants (MOP 81366, MOP 106662, NSP 104015) and a Heart and Stroke Foundation of Alberta, NWT and Nunavut Grant-in-Aid. S.P.D. was supported by an Alberta Nunavut and N.W.T. Heart and Stroke Foundation Investigatorship in Stroke Rehabilitation Research.

Abbreviations

KINARM:

Kinesiological Instrument for Normal and Altered Reaching Movements

Varxy:

Variability

Areaxy:

Contraction/Expansion

Shiftxy:

Systematic Spatial Shift

PS:

Postural hand Speed

RT:

Reaction Time

IDE:

Initial Direction Error

NSP:

Number of Speed Peaks

MT:

Movement Time

MAS:

Modified Ashworth Scale

TLT:

Thumb Localizer Test

PPB:

Purdue Peg Board

CMSA:

Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment - Impairment Inventory

FIM:

Functional Independence Measure

FIMsc:

Functional Independence Measure self-care subscore

BIT:

Behavioural Inattention Task

R-MCA:

Right Middle Cerebral Artery

L-MCA:

Left Middle Cerebral Artery

R-PCA:

Right Posterior Cerebral Artery.

References

  1. Twitchell TE: The restoration of motor function following hemiplegia in man. Brain 1951, 74: 443-480. 10.1093/brain/74.4.443

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Carey LM, Matyas TA, Oke LE: Sensory loss in stroke patients: effective training of tactile and proprioceptive discrimination. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1993, 74: 602-611. 10.1016/0003-9993(93)90158-7

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Zeman BD, Yiannikas C: Functional prognosis in stroke: use of somatosensory evoked potentials. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1989, 52: 242-247. 10.1136/jnnp.52.2.242

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Chester CS, McLaren CE: Somatosensory evoked response and recovery from stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1989, 70: 520-525.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Davidoff GN, Keren O, Ring H, Solzi P: Acute stroke patients: long-term effects of rehabilitation and maintenance of gains. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1991, 72: 869-873. 10.1016/0003-9993(91)90001-Y

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Prescott RJ, Garraway WM, Akhtar AJ: Predicting functional outcome following acute stroke using a standard clinical examination. Stroke 1982, 13: 641-647. 10.1161/01.STR.13.5.641

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Smith DL, Akhtar AJ, Garraway WM: Proprioception and spatial neglect after stroke. Age Ageing 1983, 12: 63-69. 10.1093/ageing/12.1.63

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Sherrington CS: On the proprio-ceptive system, especially in its reflex aspect. Brain 1907, 29: 467-482. 10.1093/brain/29.4.467

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kuffosky A, Wadell I, Nilsson BY: The relationship between sensory impairment and motor recovery in patients with hemiplegia. Scand J Rehabil Med 1982, 14: 27-32.

    Google Scholar 

  10. de Weerdt W, Lincoln NB, Harrison MA: Prediction of arm and hand function recovery in stroke patients. Int J Rehabil Res 1987, 10: 110-112.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. La Joie WJ, Reddy NM, Melvin JL: Somatosensory evoked potentials: their predictive value in right hemiplegia. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1982, 63: 223-226.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Wade DT, Langton-Hewer R, Wood VA, Skilbeck CE, Ismail HM: The hemiplegic arm after stroke: measurement and recovery. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1983, 46: 521-524. 10.1136/jnnp.46.6.521

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Wadell I, Kusoffsky A, Nilsson BY: A follow-up study of stroke patients 5–6 years after their brain infarct. Int J Rehabil Res 1987, 10: 103-110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Rand D, Weiss PL, Gottlieb D: Does proprioceptive loss influence recovery of the upper extremity after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 1999, 13: 15-21. 10.1177/154596839901300104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Katrak P, Bowring G, Conroy P, Chilvers M, Poulos R, McNeil D: Predicting upper limb recovery after stroke: the place of early shoulder and hand movement. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1998, 79: 758-761. 10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90352-5

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hirayama K, Fukutake T, Kawamura M: ‘Thumb localizing test’ for detecting a lesion in the posterior column-medial lemniscal system. J Neurol Sci 1999, 167: 45-49. 10.1016/S0022-510X(99)00136-7

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Lincoln NB, Crow JL, Jackson JM, Waters GR, Adams SA, Hodgson P: The unreliability of sensory assessments. Clin Rehabil 1991, 5: 273-282. 10.1177/026921559100500403

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Carey LM, Oke LE, Matyas TA: Impaired limb position sense after stroke: a quantitative test for clinical use. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1996, 77: 1271-1278. 10.1016/S0003-9993(96)90192-6

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Scott SH, Dukelow SP: Potential of robots as next-generation technology for clinical assessment of neurological disorders and upper-limb therapy. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Design 2011, 48: 335-354. 10.1682/JRRD.2010.04.0057

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Volpe BT, Huerta PT, Zipse JL, Rykman A, Edwards D, Dipietro L, Hogan N, Krebs HI: Robotic devices as therapeutic and diagnostic tools for stroke recovery. Arch Neurol 2009, 66: 1086-1090. 10.1001/archneurol.2009.182

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Bosecker C, Dipietro L, Volpe B, Krebs HI: Kinematic robot-based evaluation scales and clinical counterparts to measure upper limb motor performance in patients with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2010, 24: 62-69. 10.1177/1545968309343214

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Simo LS, Ghez C, Botzer L, Scheidt RA: A quantitative and standardized robotic method for the evaluation of arm proprioception after stroke. Conference proceedings: Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society Conference 2011, 2011: 8227-8230.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Squeri V, Zenzeri J, Morasso P, Basteris A: Integrating proprioceptive assessment with proprioceptive training of stroke patients. IEEE Int Conf Rehabil Robot 2011, 2011: 5975500.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Dukelow SP, Herter TM, Moore KD, Demers MJ, Glasgow JI, Bagg SD, Norman KE, Scott SH: Quantitative assessment of limb position sense following stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2010, 24: 178-187. 10.1177/1545968309345267

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Coderre AM, Zeid AA, Dukelow SP, Demmer MJ, Moore KD, Demers MJ, Bretzke H, Herter TM, Glasgow JI, Norman KE, et al.: Assessment of upper-limb sensorimotor function of subacute stroke patients using visually guided reaching. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2010, 24: 528-541. 10.1177/1545968309356091

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Schabrun SM, Hillier S: Evidence for the retraining of sensation after stroke: a systematic review. Clin Rehabil 2009, 23: 27-39. 10.1177/0269215508098897

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Tekeoglu Y, Adak B, Goksoy T: Effect of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) on Barthel Activities of Daily Living (ADL) index score following stroke. Clin Rehabil 1998, 12: 277-280.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Conforto AB, Kaelin-Lang A, Cohen LG: Increase in hand muscle strength of stroke patients after somatosensory stimulation. Ann Neurol 2002, 51: 122-125. 10.1002/ana.10070

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Peurala SH, Pitkanen K, Sivenius J, Tarkka IM: Cutaneous electrical stimulation may enhance sensorimotor recovery in chronic stroke. Clin Rehabil 2002, 16: 709-716. 10.1191/0269215502cr543oa

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Wu CW, Seo HJ, Cohen LG: Influence of electric somatosensory stimulation on paretic-hand function in chronic stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2006, 87: 351-357. 10.1016/j.apmr.2005.11.019

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Celnik P, Hummel F, Harris-Love M, Wolk R, Cohen LG: Somatosensory stimulation enhances the effects of training functional hand tasks in patients with chronic stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2007, 88: 1369-1376. 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.08.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Conforto AB, Cohen LG, dos Santos RL, Scaff M, Marie SK: Effects of somatosensory stimulation on motor function in chronic cortico-subcortical strokes. J Neurol 2007, 254: 333-339. 10.1007/s00415-006-0364-z

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Sullivan JE, Hedman LD: Effects of home-based sensory and motor amplitude electrical stimulation on arm dysfunction in chronic stroke. Clin Rehabil 2007, 21: 142-150. 10.1177/0269215506071252

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Yavuzer G, Oken O, Atay MB, Stam HJ: Effect of sensory-amplitude electric stimulation on motor recovery and gait kinematics after stroke: a randomized controlled study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2007, 88: 710-714. 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.02.030

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Scott SH: Apparatus for measuring and perturbing shoulder and elbow joint positions and torques during reaching. J Neurosci Methods 1999, 89: 119-127. 10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00053-9

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Fukutake T, Hirayama K, Komatsu T: Transient unilateral catalepsy and right parietal damage. Jpn J Psychiatry Neurol 1993, 47: 647-650.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hiraga A, Sakakibara R, Mizobuchi K, Asahina M, Kuwabara S, Hayashi Y, Hattori T: Putaminal hemorrhage disrupts thalamocortical projection to secondary somatosensory cortex: case report. J Neurol Sci 2005, 231: 81-83. 10.1016/j.jns.2004.11.049

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Ihori N, Kawamura M, Araki S, Kawachi J: Kinesthetic alexia due to left parietal lobe lesions. Eur Neurol 2002, 48: 87-96. 10.1159/000062993

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kwakkel G, Kollen BJ, van der Grond J, Prevo AJ: Probability of regaining dexterity in the flaccid upper limb: impact of severity of paresis and time since onset in acute stroke. Stroke 2003, 34: 2181-2186. 10.1161/01.STR.0000087172.16305.CD

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Rand D, Gottlieb D, Weiss PL: Recovery of patients with a combined motor and proprioception deficit during the first six weeks of post stroke rehabilitation. Phys Occup Ther Geriatr 2001, 18: 69-87. 10.1080/J148v18n03_05

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Yoshida H, Kondo T, Nakasato N: Neuromagnetic investigation of somatosensory cortical reorganization in hemiplegic patients after thalamic hemorrhage. J Phys Ther Sci 2008, 20: 123-127. 10.1589/jpts.20.123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Sakakibara R, Kishi M, Ogawa E, Shirai K: Isolated facio-lingual hypoalgesia and weakness after a hemorrhagic infarct localized at the contralateral operculum. J Neurol Sci 2009, 276: 193-195. 10.1016/j.jns.2008.08.035

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Bohannon RW, Smith MB: Interrater reliability of a modified Ashworth scale of muscle spasticity. Phys Ther 1987, 67: 206-207.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Tiffen J, Asher EJ: The purdue pegboard: norms and studies of reliability and validity. J Appl Psychol 1948, 32: 234-247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Gowland C, Stratford P, Ward M, Moreland J, Torresin W, Van Hullenaar S, Sanford J, Barreca S, Vanspall B, Plews N: Measuring physical impairment and disability with the chedoke-mcMaster stroke assessment. Stroke 1993, 24: 58-63. 10.1161/01.STR.24.1.58

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Keith RA, Granger CV, Hamilton BB, Sherwin FS: The functional independence measure: a new tool for rehabilitation. Adv Clin Rehabil 1987, 1: 6-18.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Bickley LS, Szilagyi PG: Bates’ Guide to Physical Examination and History Taking (9th Edition). Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins, Hagerstown, MD; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Halligan P: Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1987, 68: 98-102.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Adamo DE, Martin BJ, Brown SH: Age-related differences in upper limb proprioceptive acuity. Percept Mot Skills 2007, 104: 1297-1309.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Adamo DE, Alexander NB, Brown SH: The influence of age and physical activity on upper limb proprioceptive ability. J Aging Phys Act 2009, 17: 272-293.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Langan J, Peltier SJ, Bo J, Fling BW, Welsh RC, Seidler RD: Functional implications of age differences in motor system connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci 2010, 4: 17.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, Kwak Y, Lipps DB: Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010, 34: 721-733. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. McGivern RF, Adams B, Handa RJ, Pineda JA: Men and women exhibit a differential bias for processing movement versus objects. PLoS One 2012, 7: e32238. 10.1371/journal.pone.0032238

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Gorbet DJ, Sergio LE: Preliminary sex differences in human cortical BOLD fMRI activity during the preparation of increasingly complex visually guided movements. Eur J Neurosci 2007, 25: 1228-1239. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05358.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Bell EC, Willson MC, Wilman AH, Dave S, Silverstone PH: Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks. NeuroImage 2006, 30: 529-538. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.049

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Goble DJ, Brown SH: Upper limb asymmetries in the matching of proprioceptive versus visual targets. J Neurophysiol 2008, 99: 3063-3074. 10.1152/jn.90259.2008

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Goble DJ, Lewis CA, Brown SH: Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback. Exp Brain Res 2006, 168: 307-311. 10.1007/s00221-005-0280-y

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Goble DJ, Brown SH: Upper limb asymmetries in the perception of proprioceptively determined dynamic position sense. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2010, 36: 768-775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Goble DJ, Noble BC, Brown SH: Proprioceptive target matching asymmetries in left-handed individuals. Experimental brain research Experimentelle Hirnforschung Experimentation cerebrale 2009, 197: 403-408. 10.1007/s00221-009-1922-2

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Przybyla A, Good DC, Sainburg RL: Dynamic dominance varies with handedness: reduced interlimb asymmetries in left-handers. Experimental brain research Experimentelle Hirnforschung Experimentation cerebrale 2012, 216: 419-431. 10.1007/s00221-011-2946-y

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Bernard JA, Taylor SF, Seidler RD: Handedness, dexterity, and motor cortical representations. J Neurophysiol 2011, 105: 88-99. 10.1152/jn.00512.2010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Bobath B: Adult hemiplegia: evaluation and treament. Heinemann, London; 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Wilson ET, Wong J, Gribble PL: Mapping proprioception across a 2D horizontal workspace. PLoS One 2010, 5: e11851. 10.1371/journal.pone.0011851

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Wong JD, Wilson ET, Gribble PL: Spatially selective enhancement of proprioceptive acuity following motor learning. J Neurophysiol 2011, 105: 2512-2521. 10.1152/jn.00949.2010

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Beebe JA, Lang CE: Active range of motion predicts upper extremity function 3 months after stroke. Stroke 2009, 40: 1772-1779. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.536763

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Ellis MD, Sukal T, DeMott T, Dewald JP: Augmenting clinical evaluation of hemiparetic arm movement with a laboratory-based quantitative measurement of kinematics as a function of limb loading. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2008, 22: 321-329.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Winter JA, Allen TJ, Proske U: Muscle spindle signals combine with the sense of effort to indicate limb position. J Physiol 2005, 568: 1035-1046. 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092619

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Gandevia SC, McCloskey DI: Joint sense, muscle sense, and their combination as position sense, measured at the distal interphalangeal joint of the middle finger. J Physiol 1976, 260: 387-407.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Scott SH: Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004, 5: 532-546.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Goodale MA, Milner AD: Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 1992, 15: 20-25. 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Goodale MA: Action without perception in human vision. Cogn Neuropsychol 2008, 25: 891-919. 10.1080/02643290801961984

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Kroliczak G, Heard P, Goodale MA, Gregory RL: Dissociation of perception and action unmasked by the hollow-face illusion. Brain Res 2006, 1080: 9-16. 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.01.107

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to acknowledge the efforts of Ms. Helen Bretzke, Dr. Angela Coderre, Mrs. Mary Jo Demers, Ms. Megan Metzler, Ms. Kim Moore, Mr. Justin Peterson, Ms. Sarah Snow, Mrs. Janice Yajure for technical support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean P Dukelow.

Additional information

Competing interests

SHS is the co-founder and scientific officer of BKIN technologies, the company which commercializes the KINARM robotic device. The other authors SPD, TMH and SDB have no competing interests to declare.

Authors’ contributions

SPD participated in the design of the study, data collection, data analysis and drafting the manuscript. TMH carried out data analysis and assisted with drafting of the manuscript. SDB was involved with patient recruitment and drafting of the manuscript. SHS was involved with design and coordination of the study, data analysis and drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Authors’ original file for figure 3

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dukelow, S.P., Herter, T.M., Bagg, S.D. et al. The independence of deficits in position sense and visually guided reaching following stroke. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 9, 72 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-9-72

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-9-72

Keywords