Skip to main content

Dual task interference during gait in patients with unilateral vestibular disorders

Abstract

Background

Vestibular patients show slower and unsteady gait; they have also been shown to need greater cognitive resources when carrying out balance and cognitive dual tasks (DT). This study investigated DT interference during gait in a middle-aged group of subjects with dizziness and unsteadiness after unilateral vestibular neuronitis and in a healthy control group.

Methods

Fourteen individuals with subacute unilateral vestibular impairment after neuronitis and seventeen healthy subjects performed gait and cognitive tasks in single and DT conditions. A statistical gait analysis system was used and spatio-temporal parameters were considered. The cognitive task, consisting of backward counting by three, was tape recorded and the number of right figures was then calculated.

Results

Both patients and controls showed a more conservative gait during DT and between groups significant differences were not found. A significant decrease in cognitive performance during DT was found only in the vestibular group.

Conclusions

Results suggest that less attentional resources are available during gait in vestibular patients compared to controls, and that a priority is given in keeping up the motor task to the detriment of a decrease of the cognitive performance during DT.

Background

Vestibular information is important during gait together with vision and somatosensory input [1]. In fact, galvanic vestibular stimulation, experimentally producing functional imbalance between the two vestibular apparatus, causes in healthy subjects gait deviation to the anodal side [2]. Moreover, gait unsteadiness is a common complaint after vestibular neuritis and a higher fall risk has been also demonstrated in these patients [3] who often need to undertake a vestibular rehabilitation program in order to promote functional recovery.

Previous studies have shown in these patients spatio-temporal gait changes, disruption of head-trunk coordination and decrease of head movements [4–9]. Furthermore, the presence of a cognitive-vestibular interaction and the complaint of poor concentration, memory impairment [10] and disrupted navigation [11] has been also highlighted in vestibular patients.

The dual task (DT) paradigm, by simultaneously employing balance and cognitive tasks, has been used in recent years to shed light on motor and cognitive interference [12] in healthy young people [13, 14], elderly subjects [15] and patients. In particular, cognitive and motor interference during gait has been studied in pathologies such as stroke [16], Parkinson disease [17] and dementia [18]. These studies are based on the assumption that cognitive resources are limited and can be undermined by the execution of concurrent tasks, especially when pathologies limit the performance by a reduction of attentional resources available or by the need of increased attention to carry out usual tasks owing to motor-sensory impairments. Moreover, the degree of impairment of motor-sensory integration during DT will depend on the amount of attentional resources required by the proposed tasks [14, 19].

Motor-cognitive interference has been previously demonstrated during standing balance tasks in vestibular patients. In fact, vestibular impairments require increased attention for postural and balance control also in a compensated phase. However to our knowledge, the possible DT interference during a common but more attention requiring condition such as gait, has never been studied before in vestibular patients. This issue is of particular interest in the subacute phase, when vestibular rehabilitation programs are usually carried out to improve dynamic balance which in the activities of daily living are commonly challenged by DT.

The main aim of this study was to explore DT interference in a homogeneous group of patients with unilateral vestibular impairment while performing backward counting during gait, comparing the performances during single and DT. A second aim was to find out differences if any with an healthy control group. Since gait and balance impairments increase with age, together with the need of attentional resources, causing more DT interferences [12], older adults were not recruited in this study.

Methods

Participants

Patients and control subjects were included if their age was between 18 and 65 years and without other pathologies interfering with gait, balance and cognition. 14 patients with unilateral vestibular impairment after vestibular neuritis and 17 healthy control subjects were evaluated. Mean age, height and weight of the patient group, made up of 8 female and 6 male, were 45.2 ± 8.1 years, 169 ± 9.8 cm and 66.4 ± 13.4 Kg while the values of the control group, made up of 13 female and 4 male, were 44.1 ± 7.9 years, 167 ± 10 cm. and 69.8 ± 14.3 Kg respectively. The two groups did not result statistically different for the aforementioned parameters. All patients enrolled were referred for vestibular rehabilitation for enduring complaints of dizziness or unbalance caused by the previous neuritis. The mean time elapsed from the acute phase was 5.1 ± 6.2 months (range 1-24) and nervous system suppressant medications were no more used at the time of the assessment. The patients were first assessed by a neurotologist and underwent alternating binaural bithermal caloric stimulation, head positioning test and audiometry. Vestibular tests showed 7 patients with right and 3 with left hypofunction, 2 with right and 2 with left loss of vestibular function. Brain MRI was also carried out in order to rule out other vestibular or central nervous system pathologies that can affect balance such as cerebello-pontine angle or white matter diseases. As these tests did not demonstrate any other pathologies, the diagnosis of peripheral unilateral vestibular impairment was confirmed.

All patients filled in a Dizziness Handicap Inventory (DHI) questionnaire for disability assessment (median 50, range 20-80).

All subjects signed a written informed consent and the study was approved by the Orbassano A.S.O. San Luigi Gonzaga Ethical Committee, conforming with Geneva convention.

Tasks and procedures

The motor single task consisted of walking at self selected speed, back and forth in a well-lit gait laboratory without stopping. The distance between each turn was of 12 m. The cognitive single task consisted of backward counting aloud by 3 while the subjects were comfortably seated. During DT participants were asked to walk while backward counting without any prioritization of cognitive or motor task, and to carry out the test to the best of their abilities. In case of miscalculations, they were instructed to continue counting from the last spoken digit.

Each participant performed three trials. Before the beginning of the test, participants carried out a walking practice trial of about 1 minute wearing gait analysis sole sensors and performed a brief counting practice, showing to have understood the task. The order of the trials was randomised and each trial lasted one minute. Each participant underwent all test conditions during the same day.

STEP 32 gait analysis system (DEM Italia, Leinì, Turin, Italy) was employed for gait assessment. Phases of contact of the feet with the ground on a three level scale (heel, sole and forefoot) were acquired from three adhesive footswitches placed under the first and fifth metatarsal head and under the posterior part of the heel of each bare foot. The acquired data were offline statistically processed by the system software. The sampling rate was of 2 KHz and footswitches closing strength was of 3 N. The statistical gait analysis system employed allows atypical gait patterns such as those recorded during turns and acceleration-deceleration phases to be excluded from analysis. The parameters evaluated were: foot contact (FC), defined as the stride phase percentage in which all three foot switches were in contact with the ground, swing (Sw) and double support (DS) percentages of stride, stride time (ST) and coefficient of variance of stride time (CV). CV was calculated as the percentage of the quotient between standard deviation and stride time mean (CV = [SD/ST mean] × 100). For each gait parameter the mean value of right and left sides was calculated and used for statistical analysis.

The cognitive task consisted of backward counting by three, starting from 300. Previous studies demonstrated this cognitive task as sufficiently challenging in DT even in standing condition [14]. Single cognitive task and DT were tape recorded and the number of correct calculations were considered for further analysis.

Data analysis

Mean values and standard deviations (SD) were used for gait parameters and cognitive performance. A paired t-test was used to assess gait and cognitive parameters changes between single and DT conditions within groups. An independent t-test was employed for between groups gait parameters comparisons in single and DT conditions. Owing to small sample groups separate t-test were chosen instead of ANOVA analysis and between groups analysis for the cognitive variable was not carried out for the same reason.

Normality of data distribution was verified with curtosis and skewness tests. When data distribution was not normal, Wilcoxon or Mann Whitney test were used accordingly. A value of p < 0.05 was deemed significant. Considering that 22 statistical tests were carried out, a Bonferroni correction was made to compensate for alpha inflation and a test-wise p < 0.002 was then accepted. The statistics were performed using Statgraphics Centurion software, release XV.

Results

All patients and controls carried out the tasks properly. The analysis of gait parameters (Table 1) showed a significant increase of FC (patients p < 0.0005, controls p < 0.002), DS (patients p < 0.0001, controls p < 0.0007) and a significant decrease of Sw (patients p < 0.0005, controls p < 0.001) from single task to DT in both groups. Even though patients had higher mean values of FC, DS, ST and CV and lower mean values of Sw in both single and DT conditions than controls, these differences did not result significant when between-group comparisons were performed.

Table 1 Gait parameters values in each task by group, mean ± standard deviation.

For both groups, mean values of number of correctly enumerated figures during the cognitive task were higher under single task (patients: 22.85 ± 10.90, controls: 22.47 ± 9.28) than DT (patients:17 ± 9.33, controls: 19.76 ± 9.04). The total number of enumerated figures was also higher under single task (patients: 23.64 ± 10,53, controls: 23,71 ± 8,7) than DT (patients: 18.57 ± 8.83, controls: 21.18 ± 8.11). The within group differences between single and DT were significant for the patients group (p < 0.001) but not for controls (p = 0.034).

Discussion

This study investigated cognitive and motor interference during gait in vestibular patients complaining of persistent dizziness and unsteadiness after unilateral vestibular neuritis. We decided to exclude from the study elderly subjects in order to rule out the effects of aging as previously suggested [20]. The main finding was that patients demonstrated a significant worsening of backward counting during gait, compared to backward counting in single task (carried out while seating), whereas the control group showed only a non significant trend in this direction. In regard to the gait task, both groups had a more conservative gait during DT. Moreover, the vestibular group showed a trend of higher mean values for FC, DS, ST and CV and lower values for Sw in both single and DT conditions than controls, even if between groups differences were not statistically significant.

The decreased cognitive performance of patients in DT suggests that more attentional resources are needed to cope with an unbalanced vestibular input during gait, causing motor-sensory integration disruption. In a condition of perceptual difficulty and cognitive overload, available attention is prioritized towards the gait task which is not significantly hindered, to the detriment of the cognitive task. In both groups the finding of significant gait changes of FC, DS and Sw between single and DT indicates the use of a more conservative gait when a demanding cognitive task is overloading attentional resources. This behaviour does not result to specifically involve vestibular patients, but it is also present in healthy participants even if with borderline statistical significance.

Our findings partially agree with previous gait analysis studies that demonstrated a disrupted gait pattern in vestibular patients. Greater foot pressure on the side of the lesion has been found in unilateral vestibular patients during gait with eyes closed [4]. Vestibular subjects were found to have increased ST and DS, lower gait speed and cadence [6, 21] and higher interfoot distance during paced gait but only at increased speed [22]. The use of a trunk strategy for head stabilization, in order to compensate for a disrupted vestibulo-ocular reflex [5, 7, 8], and a decrease of head rotations while walking in dark was also demonstrated [9]. However, in our study, the between-groups gait differences were not significant, but only a trend was observed. This finding might be related to the type of our sample that included only middle-aged patients with unilateral vestibular impairment in a post-acute phase, allowing to rule out aging effect and acute phase related major balance disruption. Moreover, the gait analysis system used recorded and statistically analysed longer periods of walking, allowing a better representation of the more consistent individual gait pattern. Furthermore, CV of ST was previously found to be increased in healthy young subjects during DT [23] and greater SD of ST was also demonstrated in vestibular patients during gait [24]. However, we did not find any significant differences of CV: this observation might be explained by an inter-subjects variability in gait behaviour in the vestibular group as demonstrated by higher SD of CV in single task.

The priority of a postural strategy has been previously highlighted in a healthy young population during a standing and visual spatial memory DT [13]. On the other hand, a backward counting task was found to be a challenging task more than digit reversal and 2-bit classification tasks, increasing sway in healthy subjects standing on a compliant surface [14].

Previous studies on DT in vestibular patients, carried out with different balance and mental tasks and sometimes including peripheral and central balance impairments, had not produced unambiguous results. However, in agreement with our results, a decreased mental performance has been often evidenced and explained by a balance prioritization strategy. Longer reaction times were found in patients with vestibular balance disorders and controls in DT, with proportionately longer times while executing more difficult balancing tasks [19]. Both well compensated patients with unilateral vestibular loss and controls showed increased sway in DT but only patients had increased reaction times [25]. Other authors did not find out any impairment of a silent backward counting task during standing in a middle-aged sample of patients with central and peripheral balance dysfunction, but rather a decrease of sway, suggesting balance prioritization [20]. The performance of a visuospatial task deteriorated in both normal and vestibular patients while performing a balance task. However, a decrement in the cognitive performance and increased sway during computerized dynamic posturography was shown in patients with minor balance impairment and controls, but less sway was found during DT in patients with major balance problems [26], perhaps because of enhanced arousal.

Conclusion

This study is in agreement with previous works suggesting a balance prioritization during DT in vestibular patients with a decrease of mental task performance. This behaviour might be the consequence of higher cognitive demands required to cope with unilateral vestibular impairment when multisensory integration and gaze stabilization [27] is needed during motor tasks. For the first time, our results extend this hypothesis to a more demanding and daily motor task such as gait, requiring greater multiple sensory integration. However, even if statistically significant, the differences of cognitive performance between the vestibular and the control groups are small and the clinical relevance of the results should be considered with caution.

Our findings, if confirmed in a greater sample, might be useful when planning DT dynamic exercises in order to improve DT performance in not well compensated vestibular patients. Further research could be useful to develop new strategies for rehabilitation and prevention of falls through tailored training in dual task exercises.

References

  1. Kennedy PM, Carlsen AN, Inglis JT, Chow R, Franks IM, Chua R: Relative contribution of visual and vestibular information on the trajectory of human gait. Exp Brain Res 2003, 153: 113-7. 10.1007/s00221-003-1633-z

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Fitzpatrick C, Wardman DL, Taylor JL: Effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation during human walking. J Physiol 1999, 517: 931-9. 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0931s.x

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Herdman SJ, Blatt P, Schibert MC, Tusa RJ: Falls in patients with vestibular deficits. Am J Otol 2000, 21: 847-51.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ishikawa K, Cao ZW, Wang Y, Wong WH, Tanaka T, Miyzaki S, Toyoshima I: Dynamic locomotor function in normals and patients with vertigo. Acta Otlaryngol 2001, 121: 241-4. 10.1080/000164801300043668

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Patten C, Horak FB, Krebs DE: Head and body center of gravity control strategies: adaptation following vestibular rehabilitation. Acta Otolaryngol 2003, 123: 32-40. 10.1080/003655402000028036

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Tucker CA, Ramirez J, Krebs DE, Riley PO: Center of gravity dynamic stability in normal and vestibulopathic gait. Gait Posture 1998, 8: 117-23. 10.1016/S0966-6362(98)00030-7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Cavanaugh JT, Goldvasser D, Mc Gibbon CA, Krebs DE: Comparison of head- and body-velocity trajectories during locomotion among healthy and vestibulopathic subjects. J Rehabil Res Dev 2005, 42: 191-8. 10.1682/JRRD.2004.01.0005

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Mamoto Y, Yamamoto K, Imai T, Tamura M, Kubo T: Three-dimensional analysis of human locomotion in normal subjects and patients with vestibular deficiency. Acta Otolaryngol 2002, 122: 495-500. 10.1080/00016480260092282

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Pozzo T, Berthoz A, Vitte E, Lefort L: Head stabilization during locomotion. Perturbations induced by vestibular disorders. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl 1991, 481: 322-7. 10.3109/00016489109131413

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hanes DA, McCollum G: Cognitive-vestibular interactions: a review of patient difficulties and possible mechanisms. J Vestib Res 2006, 16: 75-91.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Peruch P, Borel L, Gaunet F, Thinus-Blanc G, Magnan J, Lacour M: Spatial performance of unilateral vestibular defective patients in non visual versus visual navigation. J Vestib Res 1999,9(1):37-47.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A: Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 2002, 16: 1-14. 10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00156-4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Siu K, Woollacott M: Attentional demands of postural control: the ability to selectively allocate information-processing resources. Gait Posture 2007, 25: 121-6. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.02.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Pellechia GL: Postural sway increases with attentional demands of concurrent cognitive task. Gait Posture 2003, 18: 29-34. 10.1016/S0966-6362(02)00138-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Faulkner KA, Redfern MS, Rosano C, Landsittel DP, Studenski SA, Cauley JA, Zmuda JM, Simonsick EM, Kritchevsky SB, Newman AB: Reciprocal influence of concurrent walking and cognitive testing on performance in older adults. Gait Posture 2006, 24: 182-9. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.08.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Plummer-D'Amato P, Altmann LJP, Saracino D, Fox E, Behrman AL, Marsiske M: Interactions between cognitive tasks and gait after stroke: a dual task study. Gait Posture 2008, 27: 347-51. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.013

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. O'Shea S, Morris ME, Iansek R: Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys Ther 2002, 82: 888-97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Allali G, Kressig RW, Assal F, Herrmann FR, Dubost V, Beauchet O: Changes in gait while backward counting in demented older adults with frontal lobe dysfunction. Gait Posture 2007, 26: 572-6. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.12.011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Yardley L, Gardner M, Bronstein A, Davies R, Buckwell D, Luxon L: Interference between postural control and mental task performance in patients with vestibular disorder and healthy controls. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001, 71: 48-52. 10.1136/jnnp.71.1.48

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Andersonn G, Hagman J, Talianzadeh R, Svedberg A, Larsen HC: Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference in patients with vestibular disorders. Otol Neurotol 2003, 24: 289-93. 10.1097/00129492-200303000-00026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Marchetti GF, Whitney SL, Blatt PJ, Morris L, Vance JM: Temporal and spatial characteristics of gait during performance of the dynamic gait index in people with and in people without balance or vestibular disorders. Phys Ther 2008, 88: 640-51. 10.2522/ptj.20070130

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Krebs DE, Goldwasser D, Lockert JD, Portney LG, Gill-Body KM: Is base of support greater in unsteady gait? Phys Ther 2002, 82: 138-47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Beauchet O, Dubost V, Herrmann FR, Kressig RW: Stride-to-stride variability while backward counting among healthy young adults. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2005, 2: 26. 10.1186/1743-0003-2-26

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Perring S, Summers T: Laboratory-free measurement of gait rhythmicity in the assessment of the degree of impairment and the effectiveness of rehabilitation in patients with vertigo resulting from vestibular hypofunction. Physiol Meas 2007, 28: 697-705. 10.1088/0967-3334/28/6/008

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Redfern MS, Talkowski ME, Jennings JR, Furman JM: Cognitive influences in postural control of patients with unilateral vestibular loss. Gait Posture 2004, 19: 105-14. 10.1016/S0966-6362(03)00032-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Andersonn G, Yardley L, Luxon L: A dual-task study of interference between mental activity and control of balance. Am J Otol 1998, 19: 632-7.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Whitney SL, Marchetti GF, Pritcher M, Furman JM: Gaze stabilization and gait performance in vestibular dysfunction. Gait Posture 2009, 29: 194-8. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.08.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alberto Nascimbeni.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

AN was responsible for conception and design of the study, data analysis, and article drafting. AP clinically assessed and included participants, participating in the data discussion. MT and AG participated in data collection and article drafting. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nascimbeni, A., Gaffuri, A., Penno, A. et al. Dual task interference during gait in patients with unilateral vestibular disorders. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 7, 47 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-7-47

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-7-47

Keywords