Skip to main content

Impaired proprioception and magnified scaling of proprioceptive error responses in chronic stroke

Abstract

Background

Previous work has shown that ~ 50–60% of individuals have impaired proprioception after stroke. Typically, these studies have identified proprioceptive impairments using a narrow range of reference movements. While this has been important for identifying the prevalence of proprioceptive impairments, it is unknown whether these error responses are consistent for a broad range of reference movements. The objective of this study was to characterize proprioceptive accuracy as function of movement speed and distance in stroke.

Methods

Stroke (N = 25) and controls (N = 21) completed a robotic proprioception test that varied movement speed and distance. Participants mirror-matched various reference movement speeds (0.1–0.4 m/s) and distances (7.5–17.5 cm). Spatial and temporal parameters known to quantify proprioception were used to determine group differences in proprioceptive accuracy, and whether patterns of proprioceptive error were consistent across testing conditions within and across groups.

Results

Overall, we found that stroke participants had impaired proprioception compared to controls. Proprioceptive errors related to tested reference movement scaled similarly to controls, but some errors showed amplified scaling (e.g., significantly overshooting or undershooting reference speed). Further, interaction effects were present for speed and distance reference combinations at the extremes of the testing distribution.

Conclusions

We found that stroke participants have impaired proprioception and that some proprioceptive errors were dependent on characteristics of the movement (e.g., speed) and that reference movements at the extremes of the testing distribution resulted in significantly larger proprioceptive errors for the stroke group. Understanding how sensory information is utilized across a broad spectrum of movements after stroke may aid design of rehabilitation programs.

Background

Proprioception, the sense of our body’s location and motion in space [1], is necessary for coordinated movement. Our group and others have detailed that proprioception is impaired in ~ 30–60% of individuals after stroke [2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9]. Loss or impairment of proprioception can result in poor coordination [10], decreased stability and interlimb coordination [11, 12], reduced motor learning [13], and overall poorer function and independence [14]. While the functional implications for stroke survivors with proprioceptive impairments are broad, our understanding of the scope and impact of these impairments to movement is limited.

To date, a variety of techniques have been employed to identify deficits in position sense and kinesthesia of the wrist [2, 15] or the limb [4, 6, 16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23]. Here, the experimenter or device passively moves the participant’s hand or limb to a pre-determined location. The participant then indicates using a protractor or other representational map of space (i.e., opposite limb), where they feel that their wrist or limb is located. While these techniques have been successful in identifying proprioceptive impairments, these paradigms have been limited in that they only observed a single type of movement or minimal variations (e.g., different angles) on a single type of movement [4, 6, 16, 18, 22, 24, 25]. Therefore, these paradigms fail to broadly survey movements similarly to how they are produced in everyday functional activities.

The contributions of proprioception to movement execution are complex and include several factors that can contribute to changes in proprioceptive error detection and accuracy outside of the impact of stroke, including limitations of the sensitivity and detection thresholds of muscle spindles [26,27,28], sensory attenuation [29,30,31], and perceptual differences in peripersonal vs. extrapersonal space [32,33,34]. It is likely that stroke further magnifies these systemic differences and limitations; however, we have only just begun to identify and understand the proprioceptive contributions to movement execution in stroke. While current research has demonstrated proprioceptive impairment in stroke, these paradigms have limited insights across a broad range of movements, especially when we consider the broad ranging neural damage that can impact proprioception [35, 36]. Therefore, the main goal of the current study was to examine how stroke affects proprioceptive error to reference movements drawn from broad speed and distance distributions within the upper limbs. Reference movements are defined as passive movements of the more affected limb that were matched. We predicted that individuals with chronic stroke will have greater proprioceptive impairments compared to age-matched controls and that these impairments would be most salient for reference movements drawn from both tails of the distributions (e.g., slower or longer movements) [37]. By identifying proprioceptive error to a broad range of reference movement characteristics, we can better understand how upper limb proprioceptive accuracy contributes to impairments in sensorimotor function and subsequently inform rehabilitation practices.

Methods

We recruited 25 individuals with chronic stroke and 21 age-matched controls to participate in this study. The following inclusion criteria were used for all participants: normal or corrected-to-normal vision and ≥ 18 years old. Control participants were age-matched to participants with stroke. The following exclusion criteria were used for control participants: history of disease impacting sensation (e.g., diabetic neuropathy), neurological disease or injury (e.g., Parkinson’s Disease), or upper body injury affecting the limb (e.g., rotator cuff tear). The following exclusion were used for participants with stroke: multiple and/or bilateral stroke, aphasia, neurological disease or injury other than stroke, moderate to severe cognitive impairment [Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [38] score ≤ 17], or upper body injury affecting the upper limb. Handedness was determined using self-report and the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [39]. The study received approval from the University of Delaware Institutional Review Board. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation. Data included in the currently study for the control group has been previously reported on in a recent manuscript by our group [37].

Experimental apparatus

The KINARM Exoskeleton (BKIN Technologies, Kingston, Canada) was used to collect kinematics from the upper limbs (Fig. 1A) [40]. Participants were seated in the exoskeleton with their shoulders supported at ~ 85° abduction throughout the entire experiment. The KINARM allows for and captures movement of the arm in the upper limb as well as delivering mechanical loads to the elbow and/or shoulder. The distal and proximal segments of the robot were adjusted to match each participants’ limb length. Vision of the limbs was occluded with a metal shutter and a bib that draped the participant’s upper body.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A KINARM exoskeleton robot. B, C Hand position data (large graph) with insets of hand speed (bottom right) and total workspace overhead view (upper right). In B and C, the white target indicates the start target and the gray target indicates the end target. B Exemplar age-matched control participant with low Initial Direction Error (IDE = 2.57°), near matched Peak Speed Ratio (PSR = 0.99), low End Point Error (EPE = 1.12 cm), and short Response Latency (RL = 352 ms). C Exemplar individual with stroke (left side of the body is more affected, right side of the body is less affected) behavior demonstrates significantly more error across all parameters compared to the control participant (IDE = 92.7°, PSR = 1.45, EPE = 26.56 cm, RL = 1986 ms)

Experimental protocol

The task required participants to mirror-match the speed, length, and direction of a passive movement made by the robot using their opposite arm. At trial start, all visual information was extinguished, and the robot passively moved the limb by moving the arm to a randomized location. The participant then mirror-matched the movement of the robot, as it occurred, with their opposite arm (active arm). The participant then gave the experimenter verbal confirmation that they matched the robotic movement, and the experimenter pressed a key to end the trial. Once verbal confirmation was received, mirrored visual target locations of the end position (2 cm cyan circle) and fingertip cursor (1 cm white circle) feedback were displayed. Participants were instructed to move both fingertip cursors into the targets to begin the next trial. To begin the next trial, all visual information was extinguished, and the robot began the next passive movement after a randomized amount of time (400–1000 ms). The robot passively moved the more affected limb in the stroke group and the participant actively moved the less affected limb. The passively moved limb was counterbalanced in the age-matched control group (dominant arm: N = 10 and non-dominant arm: N = 11) (Table 1).

Table 1 Participant demographics

During the task, participants experienced a broad distribution of reference movement characteristics. Five reference movement distances [7.5, 10, 12.5, 15, 17.5 cm] and seven reference movement peak speeds were used [0.10, 0.15, 0.20, 0.25, 0.30, 0.35, 0.40 m/s]. Each passive robotic movement used a selected distance and speed from each distribution, with each distance-speed combination randomly repeated 10 times for a total of 350 movements. For example, a movement of 7.5 cm with a peak speed of 0.2 m/s was experienced 10 times throughout testing. The reference movement characteristics were chosen based on the range of typical day-to-day upper limb movements, as well as the capabilities of the robot. The schedule of reference movements was created to ensure that (1) each reference speed and distance was sampled equally, (2) the movement occurred in a random direction sampled from a random distribution from 0° to 360°, and (3) each distance and speed combination for the reference movement was randomly ordered. The resulting schedule was used for all participants.

Data analysis

All metrics assessing proprioception were calculated within the period from movement onset of the passive limb to movement offset of the active limb. For passive movements generated by the robot, movement onset was defined as the time where robot hand speed exceeded 10% of peak hand speed for 50 consecutive milliseconds. For active movements generated by the participant, active movement onset was defined identically, but within the active hand speed time series. If active movement onset occurred less than 150 ms after the robot began moving the passive limb, the trial was discarded due to being anticipatory in nature (Mean ± SD, Controls: 1.1 ± 1.3 trials, Stroke: 2.1 ± 4.8 trials). Movement offset was defined as the time when the hand speed decreased below 0.01 m/s for 500 consecutive ms or when end-of-trial key press occurred, whichever occurred first. Peak speed was defined as the maximum hand speed between movement onset and movement offset.

The following spatial and temporal measures have been previously found to accurately identify proprioceptive impairments in individuals with stroke [4, 6]. For spatial measures, the data from the active arm was reflected across the x-axis to make comparisons within the same space. End Point Error (EPE) was calculated as the Euclidean distance between the active hand and the passive hand at their respective movement offset locations (Eq. 1). A larger EPE indicated a larger amount of proprioceptive error.

$$EPE= \sqrt{\sum {\left({Offset}_{passive} - {Offset}_{active}\right)}^{2}}.$$
(1)

Initial Direction Error (IDE) was calculated as the angle between the passive and active limb at their respective peak speeds (Eq. 2). A larger IDE indicated a larger amount of proprioceptive error, and a smaller IDE indicated a smaller amount of proprioceptive error.

$${IDE= cos}^{-1} \left(\frac{{Initial\; Movement\; Vector}_{passive}}{{Initial\; Movement\; Vector}_{active}}\right).$$
(2)

Path Length Ratio (PLR) was calculated as the quotient of the arc length of the passive limb and the arc length of the active limb (Eq. 3). A PLR of 1 indicated perfect path length matching, and values > 1 indicated that the length of the movement of the active arm was longer than the length of the movement for the passive arm.

$$PLR= \frac{{Movement\; Arc\; Length}_{active}}{{Movement\; Arc\; Length}_{passive}}.$$
(3)

Response Latency (RL) was calculated as the time difference between the movement onset of the passive arm and the movement onset of the active arm (Eq. 4). A larger Response Latency indicated it took longer for a participant to respond to the passive movement.

$$RL={Onset}_{active}- {Onset}_{passive}.$$
(4)

Peak Speed Ratio (PSR) was calculated as the quotient of the peak speed of the passive limb to peak speed of the active limb (Eq. 5). A PSR of 1 indicated perfect peak speed matching and values > 1 indicated the active arm had a larger peak speed than the passive arm.

$$PSR=\frac{{Peak\; Speed }_{active}}{{Peak\; Speed}_{passive}}.$$
(5)

Statistical analyses

For each participant, averages were computed across movements made within each speed or distance value for each of the above-described parameters. First, we determined whether there were group (stroke vs. control) level differences within each of the tested speeds and/or distances. Here, we used permutation tests with 1,000,000 iterations and quantified magnitude of difference in performance using common language effect size (CLES) [41]. Directional permutation tests \(({H}_{0}{:}\;stroke < controls)\) were used for one-sided, non-ratio measures (EPE, RL, and IDE), and non-directional permutation tests were used for two-sided, ratio measures (PLR, PSR). This analysis was repeated for a secondary analysis in which we compared within the stroke group (left-affected vs. right-affected) with only non-directional tests. The more-affected side was determined from clinical measures. Additionally, we used a non-directional permutation test to compare ages of our two groups. Second, we aimed to examine the pattern and scaling of errors across (1) speed of the reference movement, and (2) distance of the reference movement. Here, for each participant, average values from each reference speed or distance value were computed and fit to a line using ordinary least squares. For example, for each parameter, a participant would have a single average for each of the five tested reference distances, for a total of five values (e.g., average EPE values at 7.5, 10, 12.5, 15, and 17.5 cm). These values were fit to a line using ordinary least squares resulting in the calculation of intercept (β0) and slope (β1) coefficients for each participant, with the intercept representing the amount of error at 0 and the slope representing the scaling of error across reference speed or distance. Group comparisons (control vs. stroke) using the resultant coefficient values were computed using the above-described permutation test methods for β0 and β1 to determine group-level differences in magnitude and scaling of error, respectively. Lastly, we aimed to examine interaction effects of reference distance x speed within each group to determine if proprioceptive error was dependent upon both the reference speed and distance. A two-way ANOVA (distance × speed) was computed for each group, where interaction terms were calculated from average within group performance for each of the reference movement characteristics. All alpha levels were set to 0.05 and all analyses and figures were created using custom MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) scripts.

Results

We collected data from 46 participants and compared behavior from age-matched controls and stroke participants to determine whether characteristics of the reference movement impacted proprioceptive error. No significant differences were found for age between the groups (p = 0.4, CLES = 57.33%; Control: 62.62 ± 10.61 yrs, 9 M/12 F, Stroke: 65.40 ± 12.32 yrs, 17 M/8 F). All (N = 21) age-matched controls were right-handed and N = 18 stroke participants were right-handed (N = 6 left-handed, N = 1 ambidextrous, Table 1).

Clinical differences between individuals with right/left side more affected

To determine if there were differences between participants who had a stroke that mostly affected their right side versus participants who had a stroke that mostly affected their left side, we split up our individuals with stroke by side more affected and compared their clinical measures [Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): right-affected: 24 ± 3.7 and left-affected: 26.6 ± 2.4; p = 0.05, CLES = 70.83%; Functional Independence Measurement (FIM): right-affected: 123.8 ± 2.3 and left-affected: 121.6 ± 6.4; p = 0.34, CLES = 52.88%; Fugl-Meyer Assessment (FMA): right-affected: 57.8 ± 13.9 and left-affected: 48.1 ± 18.4; p = 0.149, CLES = 72.12%; Purdue Pegboard (PPB): right-affected: 7.0 ± 4.6 and left-affected: 3.7 ± 4.0; p = 0.071, CLES = 73.40%; Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT): right-affected: 142.0 ± 6.9 and left-affected: 139.3 ± 8.4; p = 0.434, CLES = 72.44%]. Therefore, we decided to collapse across side more affected for individuals with stroke for our main analyses but separate for secondary analyses.

Proprioceptive error as a function of movement speed

To determine whether reference movement speeds were perceived differently by stroke participants, matching movements were grouped according to each of the reference speeds, regardless of movement distance. Initial comparisons were completed for group averages within each movement speed to determine if stroke participants had significantly impaired sense of speed compared to controls (Fig. 2, left column, A, C, E, G). We found that stroke participants had significantly increased error compared to controls for all tested reference speeds, for all parameters except PSR, which was significantly different for only the 0.1 m/s reference speed (Fig. 2, see Table 2 for full statistics). These results demonstrate that regardless of tested speed, stroke participants had significantly impaired proprioception across spatial and temporal aspects of the movement. To understand whether error changed as a function of tested speed, we fit data from each participant using ordinary least squares and then compared the group distributions for each coefficient [intercept (β0) and slope (β1)]. Overall, we found that stroke participants had significantly higher intercept values than age-matched controls for all parameters (Fig. 2: right column, B, D, F, H) [EPE (cm): Stroke = 6.1 ± 3.6, Control = 3.3 ± 0.8; p < 0.001, CLES = 78.9%; RL (ms): Stroke = 1747 ± 641, Control = 915 ± 660; p < 0.001, CLES = 81.5%; PLR: Stroke = 1.3 ± 0.3, Control = 1.1 ± 0.1; p < 0.001, CLES = 85.1%; IDE (°): Stroke = 24.0 ± 15.5, Control = 14.8 ± 3.8; p = 0.003, CLES = 64.2%; PSR: Stroke = 1.5 ± 0.4, Control = 1.3 ± 0.1; p = 0.02, CLES = 69.9%]. In contrast, we found that for stroke participants, PSR was the only parameter with a significantly different slope (β1) value than controls (PSR: Stroke = − 2.5 ± 0.9, Control = − 1.7 ± 0.6; p < 0.001, CLES = 81.9%). Overall, we see that the magnitude of proprioceptive impairment is elevated across all reference speeds in the majority of parameters for stroke participants, while the error response for matching the speed of the movement (PSR) scales differently for stroke survivors where they significantly overestimate the reference movement speed at slower speeds and significantly underestimate the reference movement speed at faster speeds (Fig. 2F).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Proprioceptive accuracy at each tested robot speed. In the left column (A, C, E, G), the box plots display proprioceptive error in each of four parameters (EPE, RL, PSR, IDE) for age-matched controls (black) and individuals with stroke (gray). For stroke participants, left-facing triangles indicate individual stroke participants with the left side of the body as their more affected side, and right-facing triangles indicate individuals with the right side of the body as their more affected side. We observed that across each of the parameters in the left column that stroke participants showed significantly more error compared to age-matched controls at each of the tested robot speeds. In the right column (B, D, F, H) data was fit using ordinary least squares for individual participants to determine the intercept (\({\beta }_{0}\)) and slope (\({\beta }_{1}\)) of the average resultant behavior within groups. Here, the goal was to examine changes in pattern and/or magnitude of proprioceptive error as a function of robot speed. Solid lines indicate the bootstrapped average fit for age-matched controls (black) and individuals with stroke (gray). Insets indicate fit coefficients (\({\beta }_{0}\) and \({\beta }_{1}\)), with individual participant coefficient data included on the box plot. Overall, we found that for the parameters tested, the pattern (slope) was similar between age-matched controls and individuals with stroke, but that the magnitude (intercept) was significantly different for all individuals with stroke suggesting that while stroke increases the overall magnitude, the pattern of error response is preserved when the speed of the reference movement is changed. *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01

Table 2 Statistics for within speed and distance comparisons between age-matched controls and individuals with stroke

We completed secondary analyses to examine whether there were significant differences within the stroke group for participants who were left-affected vs. right-affected. When we examined parameter error (e.g., EPE) as a function of reference speed, we found that those that were left-affected had significantly more error than right-affected subjects for EPE and IDE at each tested reference speed (Table 3). For PLR, those that were left-affected had significantly higher error for only the 0.25 m/s reference speed. These results are supported by significantly higher intercept (β0) values for EPE and IDE for left-affected individuals compared to right-affected.

Table 3 Robot speed analysis statistics for comparisons between right more-affected and left more-affected for individuals with stroke

Proprioceptive error as a function of movement distance

We then examined whether reference movement distances were perceived differently by stroke participants. Here, matching movements were grouped according to each of the reference distances, regardless of movement speed. Within distance averages were computed for each group to determine if stroke participants had significantly impaired perception of movement distance compared to controls (Fig. 3, left column). We found that stroke participants had significantly increased error compared to controls for all tested reference distances, for all parameters except PSR, which was not significantly different from controls (see Table 2 for full statistics). These results demonstrate that regardless of reference distance, stroke participants had significantly impaired proprioception across spatial and temporal aspects of the movement, regardless of the distance of the reference movement. We aimed to determine whether proprioceptive error changed as a function of tested distance and fit data from each participant using ordinary least squares to compare the group distributions for each coefficient (β0 and β1). After fitting the data and comparing between groups, we found that stroke participants had significantly larger error magnitude across (intercept) all parameters (Fig. 3, right column) [EPE (cm): Stroke = 3.7 ± 2.9, Control = 2.0 ± 0.5; p < 0.001, CLES = 77.9%; RL (ms): Stroke = 829 ± 415, Control = 424 ± 213; p < 0.001, CLES = 81.9%; PLR: Stroke = 1.5 ± 0.6, Control = 1.1 ± 0.2; p = 0.007, CLES = 71.2%; IDE (°): Stroke = 37.3 ± 21.2, Control = 25.8 ± 8.1; p = 0.009, CLES = 65.7%], except for PSR (Stroke = 0.7 ± 0.3, Control = 0.7 ± 0.2; p = 0.68, CLES = 56.8%). In contrast, resulting fits for slope across reference distances were only significant for PLR (Stroke = − 0.02 ± 0.03, Control = − 0.01 ± 0.01; p = 0.04, CLES = 65.3%), suggesting that in addition to higher overall error, measures of matching movement length showed different scaling in stroke participants where errors were higher for shorter distances and lesser for longer distances compared to controls (Fig. 3E, F).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Proprioceptive accuracy at each tested robot distance. In the left column (A, C, E, G), the box plots display proprioceptive error in each of four parameters (EPE, RL, PSR, IDE) for age-matched controls (black) and individuals with stroke (gray). For stroke participants, left-facing triangles indicate individual stroke participants with the left side of the body as their more affected side, and right-facing triangles indicate individuals with the right side of the body as their more affected side. We observed that across each of the parameters in the left column, that stroke participants showed significantly more error compared to age-matched controls at each of the tested robot distances. In the right column (B, D, F, H) data was fit using ordinary least squares for individual participants to determine the intercept (\({\beta }_{0}\)) and slope (\({\beta }_{1}\)) of the average resultant behavior within participant group. Here, the goal was to examine changes in pattern and/or magnitude of proprioceptive error as a function of robot distance. Solid lines indicate the bootstrapped average fit for controls (black) and individuals with stroke (gray). Insets indicate fit coefficients (\({\beta }_{0}\) and \({\beta }_{1}\)), with individual participant coefficient data included on the box plot. Overall, we found that for most parameters tested, the pattern (slope) was similar between age-matched controls and individuals with stroke, but that the magnitude (intercept) was significantly different for all stroke participants suggesting that while stroke increases the overall magnitude, the pattern of error response is preserved when the distance of the reference movement is changed. Interestingly, our measure of how accurately participants matched the length of the movement (PLR, F) showed that stroke participants overestimated the distance of the passive robotic movement at shorter distances [7.5 cm, 10 cm] more than at longer distances [15 cm, 17.5 cm] compared to controls. *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01

We completed secondary analyses to examine whether there were significant differences within the stroke group for participants who were left-affected vs. right-affected. When we examined parameter error as a function of reference distance, we found that those that were left-affected had significantly higher error compared for EPE and IDE at each tested reference distance compared to right-affected stroke, and significantly higher error for PLR for only the 10 cm reference distance. When we examined the overall level error and scaling across distances, we observed that left-affected individuals had significantly higher intercept (β0) values for EPE (right-affected = 2.4 ± 0.5 cm, left-affected = 4.8 ± 3.6 cm; p = 0.015, CLES = 76.3%, Table 4). Additionally, we observed that when we considered performance across all reference distances, left-affected individuals had overall higher IDE that scaled differently compared to right-affected individuals, with larger IDE at shorter reference distances and smaller IDE at longer reference distances [β0 (°): right-affected = 25.5 ± 5.2, left-affected = 48.2 ± 24.7; p = 0.004, CLES = 75.6%; β1 (°): right-affected = 0.6 ± 0.2, left-affected = − 1.3 ± 0.8; p = 0.010, CLES = 73.1%, Table 4]. We observed a similar difference for PLR and PSR, where left-affected stroke had significantly more error, and significantly different slopes indicating different scaling of errors. For PLR, we observed that left-affected stroke overshot the reference distance at short distances and converged with right-affected stroke at the longest reference distances. For PSR, we observed that left-affected stroke had a significantly flatter slope than those with right-affected stroke, suggesting a flattening of the scaled response (PLR: β0: right-affected = 1.2 ± 0.3, left-affected = 1.7 ± 0.7; p = 0.021, CLES = 74.4%; β1: right-affected = − 0.01 ± 0.01, left-affected = − 0.04 ± 0.03; p = 0.015, CLES = 76.9%; PSR: β0: right-affected = 0.5 ± 0.2, left-affected = 0.8 ± 0.3; p = 0.042, CLES = 75.0%; β1: right-affected = 0.02 ± 0.01, left-affected = 0.01 ± 0.01; p = 0.009, CLES = 78.8%; Table 4).

Table 4 Robot distance analysis statistics for comparisons between right more-affected and left more-affected for individuals with stroke

Interactions between speed and distance of the reference movement

After analyzing proprioceptive error to specific reference speeds or distances, we aimed to determine whether there was an interaction for specific speed × distance combinations. For control participants, we used a two-way ANOVA (speed × distance) and found an interaction effect for PLR (Fig. 4C, top panel, F = 10.66, p = 0.003). Here, we observed that for PLR in controls, reference movements with slower speeds/shorter distances resulted in a larger amount of error. We completed the same analysis for the stroke group and found an interaction term also for PLR, which was considerably stronger than that observed for controls (Fig. 4C, bottom panel, F = 18.85, p < 0.001). These results suggest that as the reference movement becomes slower and shorter, the proprioceptive error related to movement length estimation increases considerably, suggesting distance perception in this range is much less accurate than other movement combinations, especially for individuals with stroke.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Heat plots demonstrating interaction effects for proprioceptive errors made with the active arm at a various combinations of speeds and distances generated by the passive, robotically-moved arm. In each panel, the color and value of the heatmap indicates the group level average for the respective proprioceptive outcome measure. We observed a significant interaction effect for Path Length Ratio (PLR, C) for both control (F = 10.66, p = 0.003) and stroke participants (F = 18.85, p < 0.001), and near significance for Response Latency in control participants (F = 4.00, p = 0.054). These interaction effects, particularly for PLR, suggest that participants systematically overshoot the target at low speeds and short distance combinations and undershoot reference movements that have fast speeds and long distance combinations

Discussion

We found that stroke participants had significantly impaired proprioception compared to controls, regardless of speed or distance tested. Notably, we found that when we examined participants’ ability to match different reference speeds, stroke survivors regularly overestimated slow speeds and underestimated faster reference speeds. Further, we found that when participants matched different reference distances, stroke survivors had significantly worse performance at shorter distances, but that error lessened as the reference distance of the movement increased. These patterns suggest an amplified scaling of typical proprioceptive behavior observed in control participants.

Previous work has detailed that proprioceptive impairments are relatively common in stroke survivors and can significantly affect day-to-day function [2, 4, 6, 14]. The methodology used in prior studies to assess proprioception has typically focused on broad identification of proprioceptive impairments and has failed to survey reference movements of varying types. As such, it has only been in recent years that proprioception has been considered an important aspect of upper limb function after stroke, which means that we are only just beginning to understand how sensory information is impacted after stroke [42]. Methods that interrogate sensorimotor behavior at a deeper level, as is presented here, are critical to better understanding how proprioception is impacted after stroke, as activities of daily living require varied movement speed and distance that are dependent on task goals [43, 44]. The current study aims to build on previous work, in that the task design seeks to identify global impairments in limb proprioception, but also aims to detail whether the magnitude of these impairments is the same across reference speed or distance. Here, we test proprioceptive error across a broad range of speeds and distances to determine whether proprioception is uniformly or differentially affected depending on the characteristics of the reference movement. Information related to how sensory information is used or impacted across a wide breadth of movements will be informative to rehabilitation programs. An example of this is that it may indicate that daily functions with small movement distances (e.g., brushing your teeth) are differentially impaired compared to daily functions with large movement distances (e.g., reaching for a water glass).

Scaled error responses to reference speed and distance in stroke survivors

The paradigm used in this study allows for insight into how individuals with stroke perceive movements with varying characteristics representative of daily life. Previous work has found that individuals with stroke have impaired perception of an “average” movement [4, 6, 45,46,47]. Overall, we observed that stroke participants had significantly impaired proprioception compared to control participants. Here, we found that stroke participants had impaired perception of movement regardless of the reference speed or distance tested. Our metrics showed that individuals with stroke consistently made significant errors for each movement speed and distance with increased End Point Error, increased Response Latency, poor estimation of speed (PSR) and distance (PLR) and increased Initial Direction Error compared to age-matched controls. These results demonstrate that these impairments are not the result of a singular type of movement, as has been previously shown, but that proprioceptive impairments affect a broad range of movement types after stroke.

While control participants showed some linear scaling of error responses for reference speed (e.g., End Point Error), we found that stroke participants had increased error across parameters compared to controls, and notably had amplified error scaling compared to controls (e.g., Peak Speed Ratio). Here, stroke participants tended to significantly overestimate slower speeds and underestimate faster speeds, but performed similarly to controls for speeds in the middle of the speed distribution (Fig. 2F). This suggests that stroke participants had more difficulty perceiving reference speeds at the tails of the testing distribution (e.g., 0.1 m/s, 0.4 m/s). We observed a similar pattern for reference distance; however, when we examined the ability of stroke survivors to match the length of the reference movement, we found that stroke survivors significantly overestimated the length of the movement when the distance was short (7.5 cm) and less so when the reference distance was longer (17.5 cm) (Fig. 3F). Previous work has shown that spindles remain relatively unaffected after stroke, and that it is likely that incoming afferent signals and outgoing efferent signals are poorly integrated [48, 49]. The fact that we observe differential responses to both reference speed and distance suggests that spindle encoding is flexible and may be optimized for detecting particular movement characteristics [50].

Interaction between proprioceptive errors resulting from movement reference speed and distance

Surprisingly, we found few interactions between particular combinations of reference speed and distance. We observed a significant interaction effect for a single parameter, Path Length Ratio, where for both control and stroke participants, we observed that participants made significantly higher errors for movements that were both slower (e.g., 0.1 m/s) and shorter (e.g., 7.5 cm), suggesting that distance-based errors or perception may be particularly susceptible to estimations of both length and speed of the reference movement (Fig. 4C). Further, the lack of interaction effects observed in the remaining measurements suggests that proprioceptive performance for End Point Error, Initial Direction Error, Peak Speed Ratio, and Response Latency were dependent on either reference movement speed or distance, but not both. Interestingly, the remaining spatial measures, End Point Error and Initial Direction Error, appear to be more sensitive to changes in movement distance rather than movement speed for both groups (Fig. 4A, D). In contrast, temporal measures, Response Latency and Peak Speed Ratio, were more sensitive to changes in reference movement speed rather than distance for both groups (Fig. 4B). This difference may be due to the relative contributions of afferent fibers, where spatial parameters may be relying heavily on muscle length-based information relayed via Type Ia and II afferent fibers, where temporal information relies primarily on speed of muscle stretch via Type Ia fibers only [51]. Given that these patterns are generally preserved, but amplified in stroke participants, it is reasonable to assume that afferent information relayed to the brain is intact, and heightened errors occur as a result of poor translation of afferent information to efferent information [49].

No notable effects of hemispheric lateralization of proprioception

For our analyses, we collapsed across the more affected side for individuals with stroke. However, previous work has suggested lateralization of proprioceptive information in stroke and individuals with sensory deafferentation [52, 53]. To determine whether there were any effects of hemisphere of stroke, we reanalyzed and compared the results of those who were more affected on the left side of the body vs. those who were right-affected. We found that left-affected and right-affected stroke participants were clinically comparable on the FIM, FMA, and PPB. Notably, our right-affected group had slightly lower MoCA scores than our left-affected group; however, we must note that this comparison was just under the level of significance. Surprisingly, we observed few differences when comparing robotic parameters (Tables 3 and 4), confirming previous results [6, 54], and suggesting that for passive movement of limb there are minimal effects of lateralization. We observed in this study that the most impaired participants tended to be those who are left affected, which is consistent with previous work [4, 6].

Potential limitations

As described above, lateralization of proprioceptive function is an ongoing conversation and requires further investigation. A limitation of the current study is that we are typically unable to recruit individuals with large left hemisphere stroke due to the presence of aphasia that may hinder participant ability to understand task instruction. Future work aims to further investigate the idea of lateralization by designing tasks and task instructions that are accessible for those with conductive aphasia. Additionally, we must note that the sex of our participant sample was skewed. We tested twice as many male participants as female participants in the current study. While the effects of sex on proprioception are not definitive and several studies have found negative or mixed results [55,56,57], it is a source of potential confound.

Conclusion

Proprioceptive impairment across reference speeds and distances was common in stroke participants. We found that after stroke, proprioceptive errors can readily change as a function of movement characteristics. A better understanding of how stroke survivors use proprioception to perceive and interact with their world has significant implications for designing personalized rehabilitation strategies that address within patient variability for impairments of the upper limb.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

MoCA:

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

EPE:

End Point Error

IDE:

Initial Direction Error

PLR:

Path Length Ratio

RL:

Response Latency

PSR:

Peak Speed Ratio

CLES:

Common Language Effect Size

FIM:

Functional Independence Measure

FMA:

Fugl-Meyer Assessment

PPB:

Purdue Pegboard

BIT:

Behavioral Inattention Test

References

  1. Sherrington CS. On the proprio-ceptive system, especially in its reflex aspect. Brain. 1907;29(4):467–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Carey LM, Oke LE, Matyas TA. Impaired limb position sense after stroke: a quantitative test for clinical use. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1996;77(12):1271–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Connell L, Lincoln N, Radford K. Somatosensory impairment after stroke: frequency of different deficits and their recovery. Clin Rehabil. 2008;22(8):758–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Dukelow SP, Herter TM, Moore KD, Demers MJ, Glasgow JI, Bagg SD, et al. Quantitative assessment of limb position sense following stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2010;24(2):178–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Schabrun SM, Hillier S. Evidence for the retraining of sensation after stroke: a systematic review. Clin Rehabil. 2009;23(1):27–39.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Semrau JA, Herter TM, Scott SH, Dukelow SP. Robotic identification of kinesthetic deficits after stroke. Stroke. 2013;44(12):3414–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Semrau JA, Herter TM, Scott SH, Dukelow SP. Examining differences in patterns of sensory and motor recovery after stroke with robotics. Stroke. 2015;46(12):3459–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Sullivan JE, Hedman LD. Sensory dysfunction following stroke: incidence, significance, examination, and intervention. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2008;15(3):200–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Tyson SF, Hanley M, Chillala J, Selley AB, Tallis RC. Sensory loss in hospital-admitted people with stroke: characteristics, associated factors, and relationship with function. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2008;22(2):166–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Sainburg RL, Poizner H, Ghez C. Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. J Neurophysiol. 1993;70(5):2136–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Schaffer JE, Sarlegna FR, Sainburg RL. A rare case of deafferentation reveals an essential role of proprioception in bilateral coordination. Neuropsychologia. 2021;160: 107969.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Wingert JR, Welder C, Foo P. Age-related hip proprioception declines: effects on postural sway and dynamic balance. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2014;95(2):253–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Vidoni ED, Boyd LA. Preserved motor learning after stroke is related to the degree of proprioceptive deficit. Behav Brain Funct. 2009;5(1):36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Rand D. Proprioception deficits in chronic stroke—upper extremity function and daily living. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(3): e0195043.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Young BM, Yadav R, Rana S, Kim WS, Liu C, Batth R, et al. Wrist proprioception in adults with and without subacute stroke. Brain Sci. 2022;13(1):31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Contu S, Hussain A, Kager S, Budhota A, Deshmukh VA, Kuah CWK, et al. Proprioceptive assessment in clinical settings: evaluation of joint position sense in upper limb post-stroke using a robotic manipulator. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(11): e0183257.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Gurari N, Drogos JM, Dewald JPA. Ability of individuals with chronic hemiparetic stroke to locate their forearms during single-arm and between-arms tasks. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(10): e0206518.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Leibowitz N, Levy N, Weingarten S, Grinberg Y, Karniel A, Sacher Y, et al. Automated measurement of proprioception following stroke. Disabil Rehabil. 2008;30(24):1829–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Kuczynski AM, Semrau JA, Kirton A, Dukelow SP. Kinesthetic deficits after perinatal stroke: robotic measurement in hemiparetic children. J NeuroEng Rehabil. 2017;14(1):13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Wingert JR, Burton H, Sinclair RJ, Brunstrom JE, Damiano DL. Joint-position sense and kinesthesia in cerebral palsy. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90(3):447–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Semrau JA, Herter TM, Scott SH, Dukelow SP. Differential loss of position sense and kinesthesia in sub-acute stroke. Cortex. 2019;121:414–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Niessen MH, Veeger DH, Koppe PA, Konijnenbelt MH, Van Dieën J, Janssen TW. Proprioception of the shoulder after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89(2):333–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Watkins M, Duncanson E, Gartner E, Paripovich S, Taylor C, Borstad A. A standardized method for measurement of elbow kinesthesia. J Vis Exp. 2020;164:61391.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Deblock-Bellamy A, Batcho CS, Mercier C, Blanchette AK. Quantification of upper limb position sense using an exoskeleton and a virtual reality display. J NeuroEng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Gurari N, Drogos JM, Dewald JPA. Individuals with chronic hemiparetic stroke can correctly match forearm positions within a single arm. Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;128(1):18–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Grill SE, Hallett M. Velocity sensitivity of human muscle spindle afferents and slowly adapting type II cutaneous mechanoreceptors. J Physiol. 1995;489(2):593–602.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Wise AK, Gregory JE, Proske U. Detection of movements of the human forearm during and after co-contractions of muscles acting at the elbow joint. J Physiol. 1998;508(1):325–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Wise AK, Gregory JE, Proske U. The responses of muscle spindles to small, slow movements in passive muscle and during fusimotor activity. Brain Res. 1999;821(1):87–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Collins DF, Cameron T, Gillard DM, Prochazka A. Muscular sense is attenuated when humans move. J Physiol. 1998;508(2):635–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Goble DJ, Brown SH. Dynamic proprioceptive target matching behavior in the upper limb: effects of speed, task difficulty and arm/hemisphere asymmetries. Behav Brain Res. 2009;200(1):7–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Parthasharathy M, Mantini D, Orban de Xivry JJ. Increased upper-limb sensory attenuation with age. J Neurophysiol. 2022;127(2):474–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Makin TR, Holmes NP, Zohary E. Is that near my hand? Multisensory representation of peripersonal space in human intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci. 2007;27(4):731–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Miall RC, Afanasyeva D, Cole JD, Mason P. Perception of body shape and size without touch or proprioception: evidence from individuals with congenital and acquired neuropathy. Exp Brain Res. 2021;239(4):1203–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Proske U, Chen B. Two senses of human limb position: methods of measurement and roles in proprioception. Exp Brain Res. 2021;239(11):3157–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Findlater SE, Desai JA, Semrau JA, Kenzie JM, Rorden C, Herter TM, et al. Central perception of position sense involves a distributed neural network—evidence from lesion-behavior analyses. Cortex. 2016;79:42–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Kenzie JM, Semrau JA, Findlater SE, Yu AY, Desai JA, Herter TM, et al. Localization of impaired kinesthetic processing post-stroke. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:505. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00505/full.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Tulimieri DT, Semrau JA. Aging increases proprioceptive error for a broad range of movement speed and distance estimates in the upper limb. Front Hum Neurosci. 2023;17:1217105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Chiti G, Pantoni L. Use of montreal cognitive assessment in patients with stroke. Stroke. 2014;45(10):3135–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Scott SH. Apparatus for measuring and perturbing shoulder and elbow joint positions and torques during reaching. J Neurosci Methods. 1999;89(2):119–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Good PI. Permutation, parametric and bootstrap tests of hypotheses. 3rd ed. Springer series in statistics. New York: Springer; 2005. p. 315.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Findlater SE, Dukelow SP. Upper extremity proprioception after stroke: bridging the gap between neuroscience and rehabilitation. J Mot Behav. 2017;49(1):27–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Alt Murphy M, Häger CK. Kinematic analysis of the upper extremity after stroke—how far have we reached and what have we grasped? Phys Ther Rev. 2015;20(3):137–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Michielsen ME, Selles RW, Stam HJ, Ribbers GM, Bussmann JB. Quantifying nonuse in chronic stroke patients: a study into paretic, nonparetic, and bimanual upper-limb use in daily life. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2012;93(11):1975–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Borstad A, Nichols-Larsen DS. The brief kinesthesia test is feasible and sensitive: a study in stroke. Braz J Phys Ther. 2016;20(1):81–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Herter TM, Scott SH, Dukelow SP. Vision does not always help stroke survivors compensate for impaired limb position sense. J NeuroEng Rehabil. 2019;16(1):129.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Kenzie JM, Semrau JA, Hill MD, Scott SH, Dukelow SP. A composite robotic-based measure of upper limb proprioception. J NeuroEng Rehabil. 2017;14(1):114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Bernard-Espina J, Beraneck M, Maier MA, Tagliabue M. Multisensory integration in stroke patients: a theoretical approach to reinterpret upper-limb proprioceptive deficits and visual compensation. Front Neurosci. 2021;15: 646698.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Wilson LR, Gandevia SC, Inglis JT, Gracies JM, Burke D. Muscle spindle activity in the affected upper limb after a unilateral stroke. Brain. 1999;122(11):2079–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Dimitriou M. Human muscle spindles are wired to function as controllable signal-processing devices. Elife. 2022;11: e78091.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Crowe A, Matthews PBC. The effects of stimulation of static and dynamic fusimotor fibres on the response to stretching of the primary endings of muscle spindles. J Physiol. 1964;174(1):109–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Jayasinghe SA, Sarlegna FR, Scheidt RA, Sainburg RL. Somatosensory deafferentation reveals lateralized roles of proprioception in feedback and adaptive feedforward control of movement and posture. Curr Opin Physiol. 2021;19:141–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Naito E, Morita T, Saito DN, Ban M, Shimada K, Okamoto Y, et al. Development of right-hemispheric dominance of inferior parietal lobule in proprioceptive illusion task. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(11):5385–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Semrau JA, Herter TM, Scott SH, Dukelow SP. Vision of the upper limb fails to compensate for kinesthetic impairments in subacute stroke. Cortex. 2018;109:245–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Adamo DE, Scotland S, Martin BJ. Upper limb kinesthetic asymmetries: gender and handedness effects. Neurosci Lett. 2012;516(2):188–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Schmidt L, Depper L, Kerkhoff G. Effects of age, sex and arm on the precision of arm position sense—left-arm superiority in healthy right-handers. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:915. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00915/abstract.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Sigmundsson H, Haga M, Hopkins B. Sex differences in perception: exploring the integration of sensory information with respect to vision and proprioception. Sex Roles. 2007;57(3–4):181–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Gabby Kowalski, Kenna Gilley, and Tami Wright for assistance with data collection.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: this work was funded through a grant from the National Science Foundation (NSF #1934650) and awards from the University of Delaware Research Fund.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DTT was responsible for programming of robotic tasks, data analysis, creation of figures, and drafting and editing the manuscript. JAS was responsible for study conception and design, and editing the manuscript. JAS and DTT approve the submitted version and agree to be personally accountable for the author’s own contributions and ensure that questions related to the accuracy and integrity of any part of the work, even ones in which the author was not personally involved, are appropriately investigated, resolved, and the resolution documented in the literature.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer A. Semrau.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study received approval from the University of Delaware Institutional Review Board. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tulimieri, D.T., Semrau, J.A. Impaired proprioception and magnified scaling of proprioceptive error responses in chronic stroke. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 21, 51 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-024-01350-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-024-01350-9

Keywords