Skip to main content

Perceptual and locomotor factors affect obstacle avoidance in persons with visuospatial neglect

Abstract

Background

For safe ambulation in the community, detection and avoidance of static and moving obstacles is necessary. Such abilities may be compromised by the presence of visuospatial neglect (VSN), especially when the obstacles are present in the neglected, i.e. contralesional field.

Methods

Twelve participants with VSN were tested in a virtual environment (VE) for their ability to a) detect moving obstacles (perceptuo-motor task) using a joystick with their non-paretic hand, and b) avoid collision (locomotor task) with moving obstacles while walking in the VE. The responses of the participants to obstacles approaching on the contralesional side and from head-on were compared to those during ipsilesional approaches.

Results

Up to 67 percent of participants (8 out of 12) collided with either contralesional or head-on obstacles or both. Delay in detection (perceptuo-motor task) and execution of avoidance strategies, and smaller distances from obstacles (locomotor task) were observed for colliders compared to non-colliders. Participants’ performance on the locomotor task was not explained by clinical measures of VSN but slower walkers displayed fewer collisions.

Conclusion

Persons with VSN are at the risk of colliding with dynamic obstacles approaching from the contralesional side and from head-on. Locomotor-specific assessments of navigational abilities are needed to appreciate the recovery achieved or challenges faced by persons with VSN.

Introduction

Visuospatial neglect (VSN) is an attentional-perceptual disorder affecting 25% to 30% of persons living with the consequences of a stroke[1, 2]. It alters the detection and utilisation of relevant visual information from the side opposite to the brain lesion[3]. It is best described as a failure to report, respond to or orient to novel or meaningful stimuli presented to the side opposite the brain lesion[4]. VSN has been shown to impact motor performance in a variety of tasks[5, 6], including locomotion[7, 8]. While independent walking is one of the main goals of rehabilitation post stroke[9], persons with VSN demonstrate a poor walking recovery[10]. They show deviations in their walking trajectory[11], collide with walls and furniture[12] and present with an increased risk of falls[13, 14], making independent walking unsafe[15]. Attentional bias to the ipsilesional side due to lack of inhibition by the affected hemisphere[16], distorted space representations[17] and alack of visual exploration on the contralesional side[18] have been suggested as explanations for their colliding behaviours.

Community ambulation involves challenges of different terrains and entities that may enter into one’s walking path[19]. Dynamic obstacles, which are commonly encountered in community environments such as malls and crowded streets, are especially challenging in that they have constantly changing spatio-temporal characteristics. Avoidance of dynamic obstacles demands the retrieval and processing of information obtained from the environment as well as the planned and coordinated execution of online locomotor adjustments[20]. This requires simultaneous and coordinated functioning of attentional, sensory and motor systems, which can be compromised in post-stroke VSN. To our knowledge, the ability of persons with VSN to negotiate dynamic obstacles while walking remains unexplored but is highly pertinent to rehabilitation of such individuals[21]. In a recent report, participants who were apparently completely recovered from VSN based on standard ‘paper and pencil’ assessments were shown to display altered walking trajectory adjustments in response to changing visual motion information[22]. It was suggested that clinical assessments might not be adequate to identify deficits in processing visual motion and far space stimuli. Furthermore, a complex and challenging task such as walking may lead to the neglect symptoms becoming more apparent[23]. These observations raise the question as to whether conventional clinical assessments for VSN can explain functional performance while walking. In this study, we examined the ability of persons with VSN to detect moving obstacles (perceptuo-motor task) and to avoid collisions with such obstacles during a goal directed locomotor task performed in a virtual environment (VE). The VE provided the ideal setting given that it is safe, controlled and ecological while yielding behaviours similar to what is observed in the real world[24]. We hypothesized that in individuals with VSN, the abilities to detect and circumvent moving obstacles approaching from the neglected (contralesional) side are altered as compared to the non-neglected (ipsilesional) side. We further hypothesized that the performance in the perceptuo-motor task better explains obstacle avoidance behaviours while walking than that on clinical VSN assessments.

Methods

Sample size was estimated using GPower 3.1.2, for the analysis of variance for repeated measures with the 3 directions of approach as a “within” subject factor, assuming a large effect size (0.40) and moderate correlation (0.50) between directions of obstacle approaches. A sample size of 12 participants was obtained at a power of 80% and a type 1 error of 0.05.

Twelve participants with VSN following a first time unilateral supratentorial stroke (Table 1) were recruited from an inpatient rehabilitation centre based on the following inclusion criteria: a stroke confirmed by a CT scan/MRI; a clinical diagnosis of VSN based on the motor free visual perceptual test (MVPT) and/or the Star Cancellation test; an ability to walk independently with or without a walking aid over 10 metres; and motor recovery scores ranging from 3 to 6 out of 7 on the leg and foot impairment inventories of the Chedoke McMaster Stroke Assessment. Individuals with a diagnosed visual field defect (Goldman perimetry test), cognitive deficits (scores <26 on the Mini-Mental State Examination) or other co-morbid conditions (musculo-skeletal, cardiovascular, neurologic) interfering with locomotion were excluded. Participants varied in their comfortable walking speed with values ranging from 0.45 to 1.02 m/s (0.74 ± 0.17 m/s, mean ± 1SD). Six of them used a cane during the experiment. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation of Greater Montreal. All participants gave their informed consent to participate in the study and to publish the resulting data and patient details.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

Experimental set-up and procedures

Participants took part in two evaluation sessions taking place no more than one week apart and which included, in a random order, clinical tests, the perceptuo-motor task and the locomotor task. Clinical assessment comprised tests for visuospatial neglect (Bells[25] and Line Bisection[26] tests), cognitive/executive function (Montreal Cognitive Assessment, Trail Making B) and comfortable walking capacity over 10 m. All participants were identified as right handed on the Edinburg Handedness Inventory.

The perceptuo-motor and locomotor tasks were conducted while the participants viewed the VE in an nVisor SX60 head mounted display (HMD) (NVIS, USA). The VE consisted of a room with dimensions matching that of the physical room (12 m × 8 m). A blue circular target was present on the wall at the far end (11 m) of the virtual room and three red cylinders (obstacles) were positioned in front of a theoretical point of collision in an arc of radius 3.5 m at 0° (middle) and 30° right and left (Figure 1). The theoretical point of collision (TPC) is the point where the participant and the obstacle paths, if left unaltered, would meet and collide together. Participants were positioned at the beginning of the virtual room facing the centred target. After advancing forward by 0.5 m, one of the 3 obstacles randomly started moving in the direction of the TPC and beyond at a speed of 0.75 m/s. A fixed speed and a fixed angle of approach was chosen in order to keep the walking distance to the target consistent. The diagonal obstacles crossed the midline (straight path from starting position to the target) after crossing the TPC.

Figure 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of the virtual scene. The left panel shows the screenshot of the virtual scene with the 3 red cylindrical obstacles and the blue target. The right panel illustrates the dimensions of the testing area and the relative positions of the participant and the obstacles. The red star symbol represents the theoretical point of collision (TPC).

Locomotor task

In the locomotor task, participants were instructed to walk at comfortable speed towards the blue target. They were instructed to avoid a collision with an approaching obstacle, if any, but were not given any instructions on how to avoid the obstacle. A trial could present one of the four conditions randomly: obstacle approaching from the centre, left or right, as well as a control trial which was devoid of any moving obstacle. Control trials were used to determine baseline walking speed and trajectory in the absence of moving obstacles. In case of a collision, visual feedback was provided in the form of a flashing “Collision” sign. Participants were provided with 2 practice trials per condition and 4 to 7 trials per condition were collected, depending on endurance.

Positions of 3 reflective markers placed on the HMD were tracked by a 12-camera Vicon-512™ motion capture system (UK) and fed to the CAREN 3™ virtual reality software (Motek BV, Amsterdam) to provide the real-time update of the participants’ perceived position and orientation in the VE. Markers were also placed on specific body landmarks specified in the full body maker set of the Plug in Gait model from Vicon, with 2 additional markers placed on the walking aid when applicable. Data were recorded at 100Hz in CAREN 3™ and at 120Hz in Vicon™.

Perceptuo-motor task

For the perceptuo-motor task, participants were seated and responded using a joystick (Attack3, Logitech, USA) held by the non-paretic hand and placed at a comfortable height, while viewing the VE in the HMD. The forward motion of the obstacle was set at 0.75 m/s, a speed representative of ambulatory stroke population[27]. A forward displacement of 0.5 m triggered one of the 3 obstacles to move, or a catch trial with no moving obstacle. The catch trials were aimed at preventing anticipatory responses. The participants were instructed to press the joystick button as soon as they perceived the onset of obstacle motion, or to withhold any response in the absence of an obstacle. In the failure to press the button in the presence of a moving obstacle, the obstacle continued to move ahead and a collision ensued. The participant was not informed about the collision event. Participants were provided 2 practice trials for each condition and performed 10 trials for each of the 4 conditions for a total of 40 conditions.

Data analysis

For the purpose of the analyses, obstacles were identified as approaching from the contralesional side, the middle and the ipsilesional side. For the perceptuo-motor task, the detection time was calculated as the time taken after the movement onset of the obstacle for the subject to press the button. For the locomotor task, the minimum absolute distance was calculated as the minimum distance maintained between the participant and the obstacle, before the obstacle passed beyond the participant. The number of trials in which a collision was detected was divided by the total number of trials for each of the conditions to give the percent collision. In order to determine the presence of a collision, a critical distance was set for each participant, calculated as the sum of the radius of the obstacle and the distance between C7 and the lateral-most marker on the body or walking aid. When the distance between the participants and the obstacle dropped below this critical distance, a collision event was detected. Onset of an avoidance strategy was measured as the time at which a medio-lateral displacement (of the head markers) exceeding 0.25 m (half of average shoulder width) on either side was detected. Preferred sides of avoidance strategy were also noted.

Statistical analysis

The effects of direction of obstacle approach (i.e. contralesional, head on, ipsilesional) on detection time, minimum absolute distance and onset of avoidance strategy were examined using separate repeated measure analyses of variance (ANOVAs), followed by Tukey post-hoc comparisons with Bonferroni adjustments. Probability level was set at p < 0.05. Collision rates were compared across conditions using a non-parametric Kruskall-Wallis test. Pearson correlation coefficients were used to quantify the relationship between measures of obstacle avoidance performance (minimum distance, percent collision, onset of avoidance strategy) and performance on the perceptuo-motor task (detection time) as well as on clinical assessment of neglect (Bell’s Test, Line Bisection Test), executive function (Trail Making B) and walking capacity (walking speed). Correlations were carried out separately for each obstacle approach.

Results

VSN and Perceptuo-motor performance

Presence of VNS was confirmed in all 12 participants, with positive results on the Bells and/or Line Bisection tests. Participants scoring positive (>6 omissions[25]) on the Bell’s test (n = 4), showed 6 to 18 omissions. Those positive (error >0.6 cm[26]) on the line bisection test (n = 12), showed errors between 0.9 and 4.8 cm. On the perceptuo-motor task, there was a significant difference in detection times across directions (F(3, 35) =20.72; p = 0.01) with participants taking significantly longer times (p < 0.05) to detect contralesional than ipsilesional obstacles (Figure 2A).

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean ± 1SD values of all participants for A) detection times [perceptuo-motor task] B) Minimum absolute distances maintained between the participants and the obstaclesas well as C) the distribution of colliders (solid) and non-colliders (lined) [locomotor task] for the contralesional (CL), middle (M) and ipsilesional (IL) obstacles. The digits in the solid columns indicate the average percentage of trials in which collisions were recorded for the specific obstacle direction. * p < 0.05.

Locomotor performance

No falls occurred during the testing and none of the participants reported any discomfort or dizziness due to the VE. Figure 3 represents walking trajectories of two participants, one collider and one non-collider, in response to different obstacle approaches. Both participants showed a clear preference to deviate to the ipsilesional side, sometimes even in the absence of an obstacle i.e. in the control trials (see non-collider). The collider participant repeatedly collided with the contralesional obstacle, which caused him to stop walking, and showed no collision for the middle and ipsilesional obstacles.

Figure 3
figure 3

Representative diagram of walking strategies adopted by 2 participants. The left panel is the walking pattern of a collider while the right panel consist of walking pattern of a non-collider. Note the collisions experienced by participant #12 [# omissions on Bells test = 9] (see left panel), which are represented by the black dots, and the absence of collision for participant #6 [# omissions on Bells test = 0] (see right panel).

When considering all participants, the minimum distance maintained from the obstacle differed across obstacle directions [F(3, 35) = 8.133; p = 0.0114]. Compared to the ipsilesional obstacle, participants maintained smaller distances from the contralesional (p < 0.005) and middle obstacles (p < 0.05) (Figure 2B). This difference was maintained when the collision trials were excluded from the analysis (F(3, 35) = 9.159; p = 0.001). Five participants out of 12 collided with the contralesional obstacle and 8 collided with the middle obstacle, while no collisions occurred with the ipsilesional obstacle in any of the participants. Average percent collisions were 48.11% (12% to 70% of trials) and 49.34% (40% to 65% of trials), respectively, for participants showing collisions with the contralesional and middle obstacles (Figure 3C).

Colliders vs. non-colliders

To understand the factors that differentiate participants who collided from those who did not collide in the locomotor task, their performances were examined and qualitatively compared for the contralesional and middle approaches. A statistical approach was not feasible due to the small number of participants in each group. In the perceptuo-motor task, detection times for the contralesional and middle obstacles, expressed as a ratio of the ipsilesional obstacle detection time, revealed that colliders with contralesional and middle obstacles took longer to detect the obstacles compared to non-colliders (Figure 4). In the locomotor task, colliders maintained smaller minimum distances from the obstacles and initiated their avoidance strategies later compared to non-colliders for the contralesional and middle obstacles. All participants showed a preference to deviate their trajectories to the ipsilesional side, with no clear differences between the colliders (Contralesional obstacle: 83%; Middle obstacle: 78%) compared to the non-colliders (Contralesional obstacle: 71%; Middle obstacle: 91%). Note that for ipsilesional approaches and for control trials where no obstacles were moving, participants veered ipsilaterally in 86% and 74% of the trials, respectively.

Figure 4
figure 4

Mean (±1SD) values for A) obstacle detection times [perceptuo-motor task], B) minimum distances and C) onset time of avoidance strategy for the colliders (solid bars) and non-colliders (lined bars) [locomotor task] are represented for the contralesional (CL) and middle obstacle (M). Mean values across all participants for the ipsilesional obstacle approach are represented by the dotted line to provide a reference value.

Colliders and non-colliders showed similar results on the Bells, Line Bisection and Trail Making B tests (Table 2). However, colliders with the contralesional obstacles walked faster (0.56 ± 0.08 m/s) than non-colliders under the same condition (0.31 ± 0.08 m/s), as well as in control trials. No speed differences between middle obstacle colliders and non-colliders were observed during the middle obstacle approach and control trials.

Table 2 Characteristics of colliders and non colliders for the contralesional (CL) and middle (M) obstacle approaches

Finally, while the contralesional colliders tended to present a better motor recovery of the paretic leg and foot compared to contralesional non-colliders (mean difference of 1 unit on the Chedoke McMaster Stroke Assessment), the head-on colliders and non-colliders did not show much difference (See Table 2).

Relationship between perceptuo-motor and walking performances

No significant associations were observed between detection times on the perceptuo-motor task and the participants’ performance on the locomotor task, as measured by minimum distances maintained from the obstacle, percent collisions and onset of trajectory deviation (p > 0.57). Performances on the perceptuo-motor and locomotor tasks did not correlate with the results on the Line Bisection Test, Bells Tests and Trail Making B (p > 0.5). Walking speed during the trials was not related to percent collisions but it was, however, negatively associated with the minimum distance for contralesional (r = -0.761, p = 0.004) and ipsilesional obstacles (r = -0.878, p < 0.0001). Smaller minimum distances were associated with larger percentage of collisions for contralesional and middle obstacles (Contralesional: r = -0.6366, p = 0.013; Middle: r = -0.622, p = 0.0155).

Discussion

Previous navigation studies involving persons with VSN have aimed at understanding trajectories of walking[11, 28, 29], object recognition[30, 31] and collision with static objects present on the side of the walking path[12, 29]. A significant body of literature is also concerned with computer based navigation tasks, where the challenges of locomotion itself are not present[20, 30, 32]. The present study adds to previous knowledge by addressing a functional task commonly encountered in daily life using a locomotor-specific evaluation and by investigating the perceptuo-motor and locomotor factors affecting obstacle avoidance abilities. Our results demonstrate, for the first time, that persons with VSN are at greater risk of colliding with moving obstacles approaching contralesionally and from straight ahead, as opposed to obstacles approaching ipsilesionally. Colliders, while displaying a similar severity of neglect on clinical assessments compared to non colliders, take longer to identify approaching obstacles and display altered steering behaviours. The implication of such findings as well as the contribution of perceptual and locomotor factors are discussed below.

VSN is associated withhigh rates of collisions with moving obstacles

One of the most striking findings of this study is that up to 67% of participants (8 out of 12 participants) collided with either or both the contralesional and the head on obstacle, with collisions occurring in almost 1 out of 2 trials in some of the participants. While persons with VSN are reported to bump into stationary objects[12, 33], present collision rates with moving obstacles cannot be compared with previous studies given that collision rates are typically not reported. These high collision ratesmaycompromise safety while walking in community environments where moving obstacles are present. Limited community ambulation, in return, may further delay the recovery of independent walking[10] and reduce quality of life[34]. These observations highlight the importance of addressing obstacle avoidance abilities in persons with VSN.

Interaction of perceptual and locomotor factors

The perceptual deficits in our participants were evident through larger detection times and subsequent delays in onset of avoidance strategy for the contralesional and middle obstacles. These variables also differentiated the colliders from the non-colliders. Since the joystick was held with the non-paretichand, the results were not biased by the presence of any upper-extremity motor impairment. Moreover, due to the task being a simple joystick-button click, we believe that the handedness would not invalidate the results. Similar to other studies in VSN[35, 36], a gradient of increasing detection times was observed from the ipsilesional to the contralateral visual field. Minimum distances from the obstacles maintained by participants in the present study were also smaller for contralesional and middle approaches, suggesting that their ‘personal space’, defined as the perceived safe distance an individual maintains from another object/person while walking[24, 37], is contracted on the contralesional side. Other possible explanations include an altered internal representation of space that is compressed towards the ipsilesional side[38], an altered sense of position with respect to objects (egocentric coordinates) located in the neglected field[39] and an ipsilesional shift of the subjective midline[11] which could cause the contralesional and the middle obstacle to remain unattended in the contralesional field.

Healthy young and elderly individuals[40], and individuals with stroke but no VSN[41], tested on a similar obstacle-avoidance paradigm, were shown to increase their ‘safety margins’ when additional attentional challenges were introduced. Older adults were also shown to slow down their gait when confronted with moving obstacles[40]. Conversely, VSN participants in the present study maintained smaller distances frommiddle and contralesional obstacles. They also maintained walking speeds similar to that adopted during the control trials where no obstacles were approaching. This absence of an adaptive response to a perceived threat is consistent with an attentional-perceptual disorder that is characteristic of VSN[3, 4].

Although no direct relationships were observed between collision rates and gait speed, faster walkers maintained smaller minimum distances compared to slower walkers for the diagonally approaching obstacles. Furthermore, a qualitative comparison of colliders vs. non-colliders revealed that for the contralesional approach, colliders displayed faster walking speeds and higher level of lower-extremity motor recovery. It is interesting to note that these observations contrast with the common presumption that persons with slower walking speeds or poorer motor recovery present with a compromised walking capacity and should be at higher risk of collisions. We hypothesize that slow walking may have served as a protection by allowing diagonal obstacles passing in front of the participants, therefore preventing a collision. Slow walking speed, however, was not a ‘strategy’ or context-specific adaptation adopted by the non-colliders since their speeds were similar in the control trials. The unintentional protection offered by the fixed-speed obstacle to the slower walkers can be viewed as a limitation of the experimental design. We predict that greater collision rates may have been observed for the contralesionally approaching obstacles, had the obstacle speeds matched the walking speeds. Also, this protective effect cannot operate for middle obstacles where directional changes of the walking trajectory are required to avoid collision.

Given the absence of a comparison group of non-VSN stroke participants, one may debate whether the altered perceptuo-motor and locomotor strategies observed in the present study are attributed to VSN, or to stroke-related sensorimotor deficits. In another study from our laboratory (Aravind. G, Lamontagne. A: A virtual reality based navigation task to unveil obstacle avoidance performance in individuals with visuospatial neglect, In preparation)[41], participants with VSN were evaluated on a joystick-driven obstacle avoidance task, using their non-paretic hand to manipulate the joystick while seated. In such context that minimized postural and locomotor demands, participants demonstrated collisions with contralesional and middle obstacles, as in the locomotor task described in this study. This observation supports the hypothesis that attentional-perceptual deficits of VSN influence obstacle avoidance abilities. Rates of collision in the joystick-driven task (21% to 26%), however, were smaller than those observed during walking. This may be due to the influence of stroke-related sensorimotor impairments on locomotion and defective sensorimotor integration processes (for a review[42]), as well as to the increased complexity of the locomotor task that results in VSN becoming more apparent[4345]. Therefore, the additional burden of locomotion may make the task more complex, increasing the rate of collisions.

Additionally, Darekar et al.[41], using a similar paradigm with obstacles approaching from the middle, ipsilesional and contralesional directions, have shown that individuals with stroke without VSN demonstrated) no collisions with any of the three obstacles and ii) a tendency to maintain larger distances from obstacles compared to healthy controls, a behaviour that is contrasting to our participants with VSN. Thus the presence of sensorimotor deficits post-stroke alone cannot explain the tendency to collide with moving objects.

Need for task-specific assessments of ambulation abilities

Contrary to our expectations, no associations were observed between the participants’ performance on the locomotor task and that on the perceptuo-motor task. This is somewhat surprising given that colliders performed worse, on average, compared to non-colliders on the perceptuo-motor task. We suggest that the participants’ locomotor and perceptuo-motor abilities have interacted in generating altered obstacles avoidance strategies, a hypothesis that may be further verified in a larger sample of participants. The perceptuo-motor task also differed from the locomotor tasks in that the participants were seated and responded with a single-alternative button press, facing none of the complex locomotor demands. Persons with VSN may prioritize the limited attentional resources to the control of walking, hence compromising the attention diverted to extrinsic stimuli[11]. Responses on the perceptuo-motor task may not entirely reflect perception while walking.

A lack of relationship was also observed between the participants’ performance on the laboratory tasks and clinical scores of VSN, which support previous observations that paper-pencil tests fail to predict performance on visually-guided functional tasks[21, 23]. These clinical tests are limited to near space[46] and static visual stimuli[22] and they lose sensitivity for milder cases[47] with many of them being originally designed for visual attention and cognitive assessments rather than VSN[48]. Therefore it is essential to carry out a functional, task-specific assessment to appreciate the recovery achieved and the challenges faced by the individuals. The obstacle avoidance behaviours observed in the VE are closely related to the real-world strategies[24, 49]. Therefore, the performance of individuals with VSN in our study can provide information regarding their safety during community ambulation, lending support to the external validity of our findings. This experimental paradigm can be used to assess and potentially train individuals with neglect after stroke to avoid moving obstacles while walking.

Conclusions

Persons with post-stroke VSN show a delayed perception and experience collisions with obstacles approaching from the contralesional side and from straight ahead, as opposed to obstacle approaching from the ipsilesional side. The longer obstacle detection times in the colliders compared to non-colliders suggest that attentional-perceptual deficits, along with sensorimotor impairments and altered sensorimotor integration processes due to the stroke, influence obstacle avoidance strategies and lead to collisions. The failure of clinical tests of VSN to predict the participants’ performance on the obstacle avoidance task emphasizes the need for a task-specific assessment of ambulation abilities.

Abbreviations

VSN:

Visuospatial neglect

VE:

Virtual environment

CT:

Computerised Tomography

MRI:

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

MVPT:

Motor free visual perceptual test

SD:

Standard deviation

HMD:

Helmet mounted display

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance.

References

  1. Appelros P, Karlsson GM, Seiger A, Nydevik I: Neglect and anosognosia after first-ever stroke: incidence and relationship to disability. J Rehabil Med 2002,34(5):215-220. 10.1080/165019702760279206

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, Farne A, Whyte J, Ladavas E, Frassinetti F, Coslett HB: Hemispatial neglect: subtypes, neuroanatomy, and disability. Neurology 2004,62(5):749-756. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000113730.73031.F4

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Guariglia C, Piccardi L, Iaria G, Nico D, Pizzamiglio L: Representational neglect and navigation in real space. Neuropsychologia 2005,43(8):1138-1143. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.021

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Coslett HB, Whelan H, Watson RT: Directional hypokinesia: prolonged reaction times for leftward movements in patients with right hemisphere lesions and neglect. Neurology 1985,35(6):855-859. 10.1212/WNL.35.6.855

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Edmans J, Lincoln NB: The relations between the perceptual deficits after stroke and independence in activities of daily living. Br J Occup Ther 1990,53(4):4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Cherney LR, Halper AS, Kwasnica CM, Harvey RL, Zhang M: Recovery of functional status after right hemisphere stroke: relationship with unilateral neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001,82(3):322-328. 10.1053/apmr.2001.21511

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Chen-Sea M, Henderson A, Cermak S: Patterns of visual spatial inattention and their functional significance in stroke patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1993, 74: 6.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Paolucci S, Antonucci G, Grasso MG, Pizzamiglio L: The role of unilateral spatial neglect in rehabilitation of right brain-damaged ischemic stroke patients: a matched comparison. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001,82(6):743-749. 10.1053/apmr.2001.23191

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lord SE, McPherson K, McNaughton HK, Rochester L, Weatherall M: Community ambulation after stroke: how important and obtainable is it and what measures appear predictive? Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004,85(2):234-239. 10.1016/j.apmr.2003.05.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kollen B, van de Port I, Lindeman E, Twisk J, Kwakkel G: Predicting improvement in gait after stroke: a longitudinal prospective study. Stroke 2005,36(12):2676-2680. 10.1161/01.STR.0000190839.29234.50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Huitema RB, Brouwer WH, Hof AL, Dekker R, Mulder T, Postema K: Walking trajectory in neglect patients. Gait Posture 2006,23(2):200-205. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.02.003

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Turton AJ, Dewar SJ, Lievesley A, O’Leary K, Gabb J, Gilchrist ID: Walking and wheelchair navigation in patients with left visual neglect. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2009,19(2):274-290. 10.1080/09602010802106478

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Jehkonen M, Ahonen JP, Dastidar P, Koivisto AM, Laippala P, Vilkki J, Molnar G: Visual neglect as a predictor of functional outcome one year after stroke. Acta Neurol Scand 2000,101(3):195-201. 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2000.101003195.x

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Alexander LD, Black SE, Patterson KK, Gao F, Danells CJ, McIlroy WE: Association between gait asymmetry and brain lesion location in stroke patients. Stroke 2009,40(2):537-544. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.527374

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Zihl J: Rehabilitation of visual impairments in patients with brain-damage. St Heal T 1994, 11: 287-295.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Kinsbourne M: Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies Hove. Edited by: Robertson IH, Marshall JC. UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1993:63-86.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Bisiach E, Pizzamiglio L, Nico D, Antonucci G: Beyond unilateral neglect. Brain 1996,119(Pt 3):851-857.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Sprenger A, Kompf D, Heide W: VIsual search in patients with left visual hemineglect. Prog Brain Res 2002, 140: 395-416.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gerin-Lajoie M, Richards CL, McFadyen B: The negotiation of stationary and moving obstructions during walking: anticipatory locomotor adaptations and preservation of personal space. Motor Control 2005,9(3):242-269.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Iaria G, Fox CJ, Chen J, Petrides M, Barton JJS: Detection of inexpected events during spatial navigation in humans: bottom-up attentional system and neural mechanisms. Eur J Neurosci 2008, 27: 1017-1025. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06060.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Peskine A, Rosso C, Box N, Galland A, Caron E, Rautureau G, Jouvent R, Pradat-Diehl P: Virtual reality assessment for visuospatial neglect: importance of a dynamic task. J Neurol, Neurosurg, Psychiatry 2011,82(12):1407-1409. 10.1136/jnnp.2010.217513

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Berard J, Fung J, Lamontagne A: Visuomotor control post stroke can be affected by a history of visuospatial neglect. J Neurol Neurophysiol 2012., S8: Doi: 10.4172/2155-9562.S8-001

    Google Scholar 

  23. Buxbaum LJ, Palermo MA, Mastrogiovanni D, Read MS, Rosenberg-Pitonyak E, Rizzo AA, Coslett HB: Assessment of spatial attention and neglect with a virtual wheelchair navigation task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2008,30(6):650-660. 10.1080/13803390701625821

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Gauthier L, Dehaut F, Joanette Y: The Bells test - a quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. Int J Clin Neuropsyc 1989,11(2):49-54.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Schenkenberg T, Bradford DC, Ajax ET: Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment. Neurology 1980,30(5):509-517. 10.1212/WNL.30.5.509

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Vonschroeder HP, Coutts RD, Lyden PD, Billings E, Nickel VL: Gait parameters following stroke - a practical assessment. J Rehabil Res Dev 1995,32(1):25-31.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Robertson IH, Tegner R, Goodrich SJ, Wilson C: Walking trajectory and hand movements in unilateral left neglect: a vestibular hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 1994,32(12):1495-1502. 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90121-X

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Tromp E, Dinkla A, Mulder T: Walking through doorways - an analysis of navigation skills in patients with neglect. Neuropsychol Rehabil 1995,5(4):319-331. 10.1080/09602019508401475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kim J, Kim K, Kim DY, Chang WH, Park CI, Ohn SH, Han K, Ku J, Nam SW, Kim IY, Kim SI: Virtual environment training system for rehabilitation of 642 stroke patients with unilateral neglect: crossing the virtual street. 643 Cyberpsychol Behav 2007,10(1):7-15. 10.1089/cpb.2006.9998

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Dawson AM, Buxbaum LJ, Rizzo AA: The virtual reality lateralized attention test: sensitivity and validity of a new clinical tool for assessing hemispatial neglect. Virtual Rehabil 2008, 2008: 77-82.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Weiss PL, Naveh Y, Katz N: Design and testing of a virtual environment to train stroke patients with unilateral spatial neglect to cross a street safely. Occup Ther Int 2003,10(1):39-55. 10.1002/oti.176

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Punt TD, Kitadono K, Hulleman J, Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ: From both sides now: crossover effects influence navigation in patients with unilateral neglect. J Neurol, Neurosurg, Psychiatry 2008,79(4):464-466. 10.1136/jnnp.2007.129205

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Pound P, Gompertz P, Ebrahim S: A patient-centred study of the consequences of stroke. Clin Rehabil 1998,12(4):338-347. 10.1191/026921598677661555

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Butler BC, Eskes GA, Vandorpe RA: Gradients of detection in neglect: comparison of peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Neuropsychologia 2004,42(3):346-358. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.08.008

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Dvorkin AY, Bogey RA, Harvey RL, Patton JL: Mapping the neglected space: gradients of detection revealed by virtual reality. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2012,26(2):120-131. 10.1177/1545968311410068

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Templer DI: Prison norms for Raven’s standard progressive matrices. Percept Mot Skills 1992,74(3 Pt 2):1193-1194.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Halligan P, Marshall JC: Spatial compression in visual neglect: a case study. Cortex 1991, 27: 623-629. 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80011-1

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Karnath HO: Spatial orietation and the representation of space with parietal lobe lesions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1997, 352: 14411-1419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Gerin-Lajoie M, Richards CL, McFadyen BJ: The circumvention of obstacles during walking in different environmental contexts: a comparison between older and younger adults. Gait Posture 2006,24(3):364-369. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.11.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Darekar A, Goussev V, McFadyen B, Lamontagne A, Fung J: Spatial navigation in the presence of dynamic obstacles in a virtual environment. Philadelphia, USA August: International Conference on Virtual Rehabilitation; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Aravind G, Lamontagne A: Visuospatial neglect results in collisions with moving objects even in non-locomotor conditions. Montreal, Canada: IX Progress in Motor Control confernce; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Lamontagne A, Stephenson JL, Fung J: Physiological evaluation of gait disturbances post stroke. Clin Neurophysiol 2007,118(4):717-729. 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.12.013

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Buxbaum LJ, Dawson AM, Linsley D: Reliability and validity of the virtual reality lateralized attention test in assessing hemispatial neglect in right-hemisphere stroke. Neuropsychology 2012,26(4):430-441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Buxbaum LJ, Palermo L, Mastrogiovanni D, Read MS, Rosenberg-Pitonyak E, Rizzo AA, Coslett HB: Assessment of spatial neglect with a vitual wheelchair navigation task. Instit Electrical Electron Eng 2006,8(06):94-99.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Webster JS, Cottam G, Gouvier WD, Blanton P, Beissel GF, Wofford J: Wheelchair obstacle course performance in right cerebral vascular accident victims. J Clin Expe Neuropsychol 1989,11(2):295-310. 10.1080/01688638908400890

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Robertson I, Halligan P: Spatial neglect: a clinical handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Hove: Psychology Press; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Dvorkin AY, Rymer WZ, Settle K, Patton JL: Perceptual assessment of spatial neglect within a virtual environment. Venice Italy: Virtual Rehabilitation; 2007:175-179.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Chen-Sea MJ: Unilateral neglect and functional significance among patients with stroke. Occup Ther J Res 2001,21(4):223-240.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Fink PW, Foo PS, Warren WH: Obstacle avoidance during walking in real and virtual environments. ACM Transac App Percep 2007,4(1):2. 10.1145/1227134.1227136

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants who took part in this study. We are also thankful to Anuja Darekar and Dr Joyce Fung for their assistance in the conception of the study and to Valeri Goussev and Christian Beaudoin for their skilful technical assistance. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research funded this study (MOP-77548). GA was the recipient of scholarships from the McGill Faculty of Medicine, the Physiotherapy Foundation of Canada through the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada and the Richard and Edith Strauss Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gayatri Aravind.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

GA conceived, collected and carried out the experiments. She also carried out the data reduction, analyses of the data and drafting of the manuscript. AL contributed to the design of the study, data analysis, interpretation of results and revision of the manuscript. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Aravind, G., Lamontagne, A. Perceptual and locomotor factors affect obstacle avoidance in persons with visuospatial neglect. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 11, 38 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-11-38

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-11-38

Keywords