- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Wrist speed feedback improves elbow compensation and reaching accuracy for myoelectric transradial prosthesis users in hybrid virtual reaching task
Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation volumeĀ 20, ArticleĀ number:Ā 9 (2023)
Abstract
Background
Myoelectric prostheses are a popular choice for restoring motor capability following the loss of a limb, but they do not provide direct feedback to the user about the movements of the deviceāin other words, kinesthesia. The outcomes of studies providing artificial sensory feedback are often influenced by the availability of incidental feedback. When subjects are blindfolded and disconnected from the prosthesis, artificial sensory feedback consistently improves control; however, when subjects wear a prosthesis and can see the task, benefits often deteriorate or become inconsistent. We theorize that providing artificial sensory feedback about prosthesis speed, which cannot be precisely estimated via vision, will improve the learning and control of a myoelectric prosthesis.
Methods
In this study, we test a joint-speed feedback system with six transradial amputee subjects to evaluate how it affects myoelectric control and adaptation behavior during a virtual reaching task.
Results
Our results showed that joint-speed feedback lowered reaching errors and compensatory movements during steady-state reaches. However, the same feedback provided no improvement when control was perturbed.
Conclusions
These outcomes suggest that the benefit of joint speed feedback may be dependent on the complexity of the myoelectric control and the context of the task.
Background
For individuals living with upper limb loss or difference, myoelectric prostheses have the potential to restore lost functionality and improve independence. Significant advancements have been made in myoelectric control methods, but sensory feedback is still a missing component from commercial prostheses. Sensory feedback is one of the most commonly requested features of state-of-the-art prostheses [1], and is critical to able-bodied limb control [2]. Consequently, artificial sensory feedback has received much attention over the past decade [3, 4]. Typically, this takes the form of sensory substitution feedback, where the information provided from missing sensory organs is communicated to the user via an alternative method such as vibrotactile [5,6,7,8,9,10] or auditory stimuli [11,12,13], or via direct nerve stimulation [14,15,16].
Despite this attention, artificial sensory feedback has not yet achieved commercial availability for prostheses, which may be related in part to the experimental conditions in which these systems are tested. Frequently, artificial feedback is tested with subjects blindfolded and not connected to the prosthesis. Although these studies consistently show the benefit of sensory feedback, they omit the incidental sources of feedback that prosthesis users rely on every day, such as vision, sound, and prosthesis vibration. This incidental feedback often serves the same purpose as the artificial feedback being tested (i.e. informing the user about the state of the prosthesis), and studies have shown this incidental feedback is sufficient for some tasks [17]. Therefore, when artificial feedback is tested alongside incidental feedback, results become inconsistentāsome studies suggest discernable benefits of artificial feedback alongside incidental feedback, such as improved time to target prosthesis position [18], ability to perform object manipulation tasks [19], and coordination of grasping with the prosthesis [20], however the same and other studies also show no changes in other aspects of prosthesis use [9, 19, 21,22,23,24].
One theory explaining this discrepancy stems from the degree of precision of each feedback source. When we receive the same information from multiple sources, we merge them in accordance with their uncertainty: sources with less uncertainty are favored over those with greater uncertainty [25, 26]. Therefore, if incidental feedback (particularly vision) is more precise than the artificial feedback being tested, then the tested feedback may not meaningfully improve the users understanding of their prosthesis movements.
One candidate for sensory feedback which is not well estimated by incidental vision is kinesthesia. Prior work has suggested that limb speed, and in particular joint speed, has high visual uncertainty and can be supplemented with audio feedback to greatly reduce this uncertainty [27]. Knowledge of limb speed may aid in the formation of internal models of biological and prosthetic limb movements, and a previous study has indeed suggested that supplemental joint speed feedback may improve reaching accuracy during instances of perturbed myoelectric control [28]. However, the benefits of such feedback requires additional investigation within the context of impaired proprioception following amputation. Proprioceptive organs including muscle spindles and Golgi tendon organs are activated differently in an amputated limb than they are in intact limb; agonistāantagonist muscles pairs stimulate these organs during movement [29], but this pairing is generally absent from amputated limbs. Instead, standard surgical procedure for upper-limb amputations involves myodesis of the muscles to the end of the distal bone, preventing the normal passive stretching of antagonist muscles during movement and negatively affecting proprioception [30].
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of joint speed feedback on prosthesis control and adaptation to errors during reaching. Transradial amputee subjects controlled a virtual 1-DoF myoelectric limb and completed center-out reaching tasks under steady-state and perturbed dynamics conditions. We quantified control by measuring trial-by-trial adaptation to self-generated and perturbation-generated errors to learn how quickly myoelectric control users can update their understanding of the dynamics and adjust accordingly.
Methods
Subjects
Six subjects with transradial amputation participated in this study (Table 1), which was approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board; all experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, and all subjects provided informed consent before starting the study.
Experimental setup
Subjects sat in front of a computer monitor displaying a virtual arm. A Biometrics twin-axis electrogoniometer was attached to the upper and lower arm to measure the elbow flexion angle. Goniometer signals were low-pass filtered at 5Ā Hz with a 2nd order Butterworth filter. Two Delsys Bagnoli electromyographic (EMG) sensors measured EMG signals from wrist flexor and extensor sites on the residual limb (Fig.Ā 1a). The electrode placement was determined via voluntary muscle contraction and palpation (similar to the method used to place electrodes when controlling a myoelectric prosthesis), and the reference electrode was placed over the olecranon or on the clavicle. EMG signals were high-pass filtered at 0.1Ā Hz, positive-rectified, and low-pass filtered at 5Ā Hz using a 2nd order Butterworth filter. Data were acquired at 1000Ā Hz and downsampled to 100Ā Hz after filtering.
Subjects controlled a virtual two-link arm using the elbow goniometer to dictate proximal link position, and the wrist EMG sensors to dictate distal link velocity (Fig.Ā 1b). The virtual arm started in a neutral position on the screen and targets appears around the screen in four fixed positions (Fig.Ā 1c). Specifics for the control of the arm and the positioning of elements on the screen are the same as in our previous study [28]; however, the task was mirrored horizontally for left-side amputee subjects to align the movement of the virtual arm with the subjectās arm.
Subjects controlled the virtual arm to perform ballistic center-out reaches. With the cursor in the home circle (red hollow circle, Fig.Ā 1c), a ball (grey filled circle) would appear above one of four targets. The ball would drop and align with the center of the target 0.5Ā s after the arm left the home circle. Subjects were instructed to reach towards the target, stopping when the ball reached the target, and with the cursor as close to the target as possible [31]. If the virtual limb did not come to a stop at the end of the trial (defined as both proximal and distal links moving slower than 45Ā°/s), the ball was colored red. If the limb was successfully stopped but the cursor was not within the target, the ball remained grey. However, if the cursor was inside the target at the end of the trial (and the limb was sufficiently stilled), the ball was colored green to indicate a success.
Familiarization
To learn to control the virtual arm, subjects began each visit with a familiarization session. During this session, subjects were provided time to understand the controller through unstructured exploration (subjects controlled the virtual limb with no visible targets), untimed target reaches (subjects reached towards targets but were given as much time as needed to complete each trial), and a structured protocol comprising 32 training ballistic center-out reaches. The first 16 trials had a specified reaching order (four sets of 4 reaches towards each target), and the second 16 trials had a balanced and randomized reaching order (4 reaches total towards each target) (Fig.Ā 2a). No artificial feedback was provided during this session.
During the first visit, subjects only completed the Familiarization session. During the next two visits, subjects additionally completed a feedback protocol and a no feedback protocol in balanced randomized order. During the feedback protocol, subjects wore a pair of noise-canceling headphones (Bose QuietComfort 35 II) which played frequency-modulated tones determined by the speed of the wrist-driven distal link, where the pitch would increase by one octave for every multiple of 60Ā°/s. During the no feedback protocol, subjects wore the noise-canceling headphones, but no sound was played.
Steady-state block
To test trial-by-trial adaptation to self-generated errors, subjects completed two repetitions of 100 center-out reaches, each separated into one set of 60 and one set of 40 reaches (Fig.Ā 2b). The order of these sets was randomized across subjects using balanced block randomization. Subjects were allowed a short break between sets.
During the set of 60 trials, subjects completed four sets of 10 reaches towards each target. During the set of 40 trials, subjects reached towards targets in a balanced and randomized order. After each set, expanding window optimization separated initial trials from steady-state trials for post-experiment analysis [32].
Two quantities were extracted from this trial-by-trial analysis. Adaptation rate was defined as the proportion of error from one trial that was corrected for in the following trial. Bias was defined as the amount of error which elicited no correction on average. It describes the intended reaching behavior, but is not necessarily the same as the average reaching error. This analysis was performed separately on the angular errors of both the elbow and the wrist, and was analyzed using a linear mixed effects model investigating main and interaction effects of the target set (Same Targets (ST) or Different Targets (DT)) and the feedback (No Feedback (NFB) or Feedback (FB)). A similar analysis was conducted on the magnitude of endpoint, elbow, and wrist errors. Subjects were coded as random variables, and p-values were adjusted using Holm-Bonferroni corrections.
A second stochastic signal processing approach was used to filter inherent motor control noise and provide unbiased estimates of true adaptation behavior [32,33,34]. This analysis provided outcomes for the internal model adaptation rate and the control noise; both were analyzed using the same linear mixed effects model as described above.
Perturbation block
To test the speed of adaptation to external perturbations to the control system, subjects completed Perturbation blocks comprising 12 practice trials followed by 8 sets of perturbation trials. During each set, subjects started by making 8ā10 unperturbed reaches towards random targets. The control system was then perturbed by doubling the EMG gain, which increased the speed of the distal link and made accurate and precise control more difficult. Subjects then made 8 reaches with the perturbed dynamics, either towards the same target, or towards different targets. Each category was tested in 4 sets of the perturbation trials (Fig.Ā 2c). The order of these sets was determined randomly. The entire Perturbation block was repeated twice, yielding a total of 24 practice trials and 16 sets of perturbation trialsā8 towards the same target, and 8 towards different targets.
Perturbation adaptation of the Euclidean distance between the cursor and the target was estimated using an exponential decay model which fit a gain (\(\alpha\)), decay rate (\(\lambda\)), and baseline error (\({\varepsilon }_{\infty }\)) to the perturbation trial data [35,36,37].
A hierarchical nonlinear mixed effects model described in a previous publication was intended to analyze data from the perturbation block [28]. However, this method was not viable due to the variability of reaches; thus, an exponential decay function was fit separately for each subject, combining all data for each condition, and the coefficients from these subject-based models were compared [38].
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using R-4.0.5. Linear mixed effects models investigated main and interaction effects for each analysis, and Holm-Bonferroni corrections were made for the number of terms in each model. Deidentified raw data and code for statistical analysis are publicly available on The Open Science Framework [39].
Results
Steady-state block
Steady-state reaches provide insight into how subjects coordinate positional- and myoelectric-controlled joints during reaching tasks after adapting to a control scheme, and may be used to quantify compensatory movements in one joint arising from errors or poor control in the other. FigureĀ 3 shows the Euclidean endpoint errors (a) and joint angle errors (b, c).Footnote 1
No significant interactions were found (pminā=ā0.628), so interaction terms were removed and the models were rerun [40]. Endpoint errors were reduced with joint speed feedback available (FB meanāĀ±āSEM: 3.66āĀ±ā0.65Ā cm) compared to when feedback was absent (NFB: 4.80āĀ±ā0.97Ā cm, pā=ā0.047, Fig.Ā 3a). Joint speed feedback also significantly reduced the magnitude of wrist angle errors (NFB: 15.42āĀ±ā5.43Ā°, FB: 11.94āĀ±ā4.68Ā°; pā=ā0.006, Fig.Ā 3c), but did not significantly affect the magnitude of elbow angle errors (NFB: 8.42āĀ±ā2.99Ā°, FB: 7.42āĀ±ā2.57Ā°; pā=ā0.563, Fig.Ā 3b). There were no significant differences when reaching towards the same or different targets for endpoint (ST: 4.26āĀ±ā0.865, DT: 4.21āĀ±ā0.85; pā=ā0.819), elbow (ST: 6.83āĀ±ā2.59Ā°, DT: 9.01āĀ±ā2.92Ā°; pā=ā0.563), or wrist (ST: 12.89āĀ±ā4.88Ā°, DT: 14.47āĀ±ā5.33Ā°; pā=ā0.588) errors.
We conducted an analysis of trial-by-trial adaptation to investigate differences in adaptation rates between feedback and target conditions, and to identify possible compensatory strategies in the reach biases. Our results showed no significant interactions between feedback and target for elbow bias or rate (pminā=ā0.690), so the interaction terms were removed and the models rerun [40]. We found an improved adaptation rate during reaches towards different targets for the elbow (ST: āā0.77āĀ±ā0.03, DT: āā0.99āĀ±ā0.05; pā<ā0.001) and wrist (ST: āā0.76āĀ±ā0.10, DT: āā1.02āĀ±ā0.06; pā=ā0.010) (Fig.Ā 4b), but no significant differences for the bias of the elbow (ST: 3.63āĀ±ā0.70, DT: 3.97āĀ±ā0.78; pā=ā0.414) and wrist (ST: āā5.48āĀ±ā1.58, DT: āā6.73āĀ±ā2.07; pā=ā0.358) (Fig.Ā 4a).
No difference was observed between feedback conditions for wrist bias (NFB: āā7.68āĀ±ā1.85, FB: āā4.54āĀ±ā1.61; pā=ā0.060), but interestingly elbow bias was reduced (NFB: 4.40āĀ±ā0.81, FB: 3.24āĀ±ā0.58; pā=ā0.026). No differences were observed between feedback conditions for elbow (NFB: āā0.90āĀ±ā0.07, FB: āā0.86āĀ±ā0.06; pā=ā0.436) or wrist adaptation rates (NFB: āā0.88āĀ±ā0.10, FB: āā0.90āĀ±ā0.10; pā=ā0.794). Another interesting observation is that subjects attempted to under-reach with the wrist (demonstrated by the negative wrist bias) and overreach with the elbow (demonstrated by the positive elbow bias). Possible explanations for this reaching strategy are presented in "Discussion".
To supplement our traditional trial-by-trial analysis, we ran a secondary stochastic signal processing analysis. However, the data showed no significant change in adaptation rate for the elbow (NFB: 0.58āĀ±ā0.09, FB: 0.57āĀ±ā0.08; pā=ā0.996) or the wrist (NFB: 0.63āĀ±ā0.09, FB: 0.65āĀ±ā0.09; pā=ā0.887) (Fig.Ā 5a). Analyzing the control noise (Q) similarly revealed no significant differences between feedback conditions for elbow (NFB: 55.71āĀ±ā15.82 deg2, FB: 46.50āĀ±ā16.38 deg2; pā=ā0.673) or wrist control noise (NFB: 324.58āĀ±ā75.84 deg2, FB: 205.63āĀ±ā46.98 deg2; pā=ā0.157) (Fig.Ā 5b).
These results taken together suggest that joint speed feedback may improve the general accuracy of reaches (Fig.Ā 3) and result in less compensatory movement bias of the intact joints (Fig.Ā 4), which may translate to an improved confidence in the control of a myoelectric prosthesis. Further, the ratios of elbow-to-wrist biases may indicate a form to movement-based task optimization, such that improvements to myoelectric control can be detected in minor changes to bodily compensation within a constrained task. Reductions were also seen in elbow and wrist control noise (Fig.Ā 5), though these differences were not statistically significant.
Perturbation block
Perturbation trials test the ability for a person making reaches to adjust to suddenly changing task conditions, such as an abrupt change to the controller. FigureĀ 6a, b shows the averaged subject responses to perturbation trials. The hierarchical nonlinear mixed effects model used in a previous study [28] was unable to run, likely due to insufficient and noisy data, thus individual exponential decay models were fit to each subjectās data for each condition, and the resulting coefficients were compared. However, no significant factors were uncovered from these statistical models (pminā=ā0.533).
The magnitude of errors upon initial perturbation was not affected by feedback condition (pā>ā0.999) or target (pā>ā0.999). Final errors after adaptation to the perturbation were also not affected by feedback condition (pā=ā0.349) or target (pā=ā0.692). When final errors were subtracted from initial errors to determine the total improvement across the eight perturbation trials, this improvement was also found to not be affected by feedback condition (pā=ā0.563) or target (pā=ā0.759).
Discussion
This study expanded upon our previous work by investigating transradial amputee performance during center-out reaching tasks. These tasks require coordination of elbow angle and wrist EMG to complete the reach. This paradigm differed from other similar studies into proprioceptive feedback by using a ballistic reach paradigm, which prevented subjects from incorporating feedback into their task performance and allowed us to investigate solely the impact of feedback on improvements to feedforward control [10, 13, 41, 42]. Our results provide some insight into how artificial joint speed feedback may be used to improve control of a myoelectric prosthesis. We found evidence that subjects were able to reduce their average reaching errors when provided audio feedback encoding the joint speed of a myoelectric limb (Fig.Ā 3). We also found evidence suggesting the feedback may help prosthesis users reduce compensatory movement bias (Fig.Ā 4a). However, no significant differences were found between feedback conditions for adaptation behavior after abrupt perturbations to the controller (Fig.Ā 6).
In some aspects, our results agree with those from our previous study with non-amputee participants. Transradial amputee participants were able to complete ballistic center-out reaches requiring simultaneous control of positional- and myoelectric joints, in a manner similar to the how they may use their prosthesis in a home environment. Additionally, the same compensatory behavior was observed in both studies, where subjects would strategically under-reach with the wrist and compensate by overreaching with the elbow to minimize the distance to the target. This manner of compensatory movement is common for upper-limb prostheses (though normally demonstrated for the trunk and shoulder [43]), and aligns with the expected optimal reaching strategy to minimize endpoint errorāgiven that only one limb configuration can reach each target, any wrist error can be optimally compensated with an opposing elbow adjustment of half the magnitude. It should be noted that, if reaching tasks required wrist flexion instead of wrist extension, the optimal reaching strategy to compensate for excessive flexion would be to overreach with the elbow. We also observe a more positive wrist error when reaching towards Target 3 (involving simultaneous extension of both the wrist and the elbow) than towards other targets, though interestingly the endpoint errors were lowest.Footnote 2 This tendency may suggest that fine control of wrist extension is more difficult when coupled with simultaneous elbow extension. Alternatively, it may suggest that visual estimation of the requirement movement to achieve the target is more difficult. Interestingly, while we showed no impact of sensory feedback on the average errors in the previous study[28], amputee reaches in the present study demonstrated lower elbow bias, and a trend towards lower wrist bias, with feedback available (Fig.Ā 4a).
The present study differs from the previous study with respect to steady-state errors; while no significant differences were observed in endpoint, elbow, or wrist errors for non-amputee reaches, transradial amputee endpoint and wrist angle errors were significantly improved with joint speed feedback. Furthermore, the stochastic analysis reveals an interesting difference between non-amputee and transradial amputee reaches: while elbow control noise is roughly equivalent between populations, the control noise of the myoelectric wrist can be more than twice as high for transradial amputees compared to non-amputees [28] (Fig.Ā 5b).
However, where non-amputees demonstrated improved reaching errors after adapting to perturbations while reaching towards changing targets, transradial amputees showed no significant differences in perturbation adaptation behavior. One possible explanation for these inconclusive results stems from the heightened control noise. With myoelectric control noise for transradial amputees nearly double that of non-amputees, likely due to lack of or damage to proprioceptive organs, it is possible that this increased control noise led to increased internal model uncertainty, decreasing the capacity to adapt to perturbations. These trends may extend to adaptation behavior after control system perturbation. It should be noted that no individuals with congenital limb difference were included in this study; we expect control noise to be between those of transradial and non-amputees due to the more natural insertion of residual muscles, however this remains to be investigated.
The absence of incidental feedback may also contribute to the increased control noise observed in this study. When controlling a myoelectric prosthesis, users will generally rely on cues such as the sound and vibration of the hand as an indirect indicator of speed; the presence of these incidental cues in daily life may reduce control noise and consequently give rise to differences in adaptation behavior. In contrast, the auditory feedback modality used in this study provides a ābest-caseā scenario for low feedback uncertainty [27]; consequently, one might expect increased control noise when using vibrotactile or electrotactile feedback modalities with higher sensory uncertainty. This may even be the case when discrete vibrotactile feedback is fused with auditory feedback, as was seen in a recent study by Engels et al. [13].
Analyses in our current study were limited by the analysis methods available and the data collected for each. Our protocol required subjects to reach for several targets arranged throughout the reaching space, which ensured reaching performance was not localized to any one particular region. However, this also required splitting up reaches into smaller blocks of consistent reaches to prevent subject fatigue. As a result, adaptation models for self-generated errors were fit on relatively small amounts of data; this was especially the case for the stochastic signal processing analysis. Furthermore, this analysis requires a stationary target, thus reaches towards changing targets had to be omitted from this analysis. Analyzing self-generated error adaptation using two different methods allowed us to partially account for the limited data and build a fuller picture of adaptation behavior at steady-state.
The hierarchical model used our previous study requires sufficient data to fit all parameters across all included perturbation conditions [28]. Although the intent was to use the same model in this study, the smaller number of subjects prevented this model from converging. Furthermore, constraining the model parameters using insights from steady-state errors did not alleviate issues with model convergence [44]. In its place, we took an approach previously used in our pilot study [38]. In this approach, an individual exponential decay model is fit to each subject, for each condition. The coefficients from these models were then analyzed using a linear mixed effects model. To supplement this analysis, post-hoc comparisons were made on the initial and final errors achieved during perturbation trials. However, no significant differences were found during perturbation trials, whereas differences were found for non-amputee reaches.
The outcomes from the stochastic signal processing techniques also warrant additional attention. The non-improvement in the adaptation rate of the EMG-controlled wrist internal model is opposite of what is expected from reduced wrist noise. A possible explanation is that the high EMG control noise for transradial amputees, more than double than that of non-amputees at times, was more substantial than effects of volitional adaptation, which may have influenced the internal model adaptation rate as calculated using analytical methods [33]. It should be reiterated this analysis was conducted on relatively small amounts of data, which may disproportionately affect the variability or biases of calculated internal model adaptation rate.
The findings in this study corroborate those in a recent study on the clinical relevance of artificial feedback [19]. They conclude that the benefit of sensory feedback depends on the complexity of the task and the proficiency of the feedforward control. Our study involves a simple taskācenter-out reachingāmade complicated by the control scheme. Our pilot study with trans-humeral amputees used a more difficult control scheme, and the high control noise made control (and adaptation) difficult [38] However, in our present experiment with trans-radial amputees, we show that improved feedback can reduce the control noise, thereby improving feedforward control [34]. This outcome suggests a need to test artificial sensory feedback systems with amputee patients of different levels to determine how beneficial feedback is to each population. Developing a more complete understanding of which factors determine the degree of benefit for prosthesis feedback can help researchers develop clinically impactful artificial sensory feedback which improves quality of life for people with amputations.
Availability of data and materials
All raw data and code for the experimental protocol, data analysis, and statistical analysis are freely available on the Open Science Framework [39].
Notes
Abbreviations
- DoF:
-
Degree-of-freedom
- EMG:
-
Electromyography
- NFB:
-
No Feedback
- FB:
-
Feedback
- ST:
-
Same Target
- DT:
-
Different Target
References
Cordella F, et al. Literature review on needs of upper limb prosthesis users. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:1ā14.
Miall RC, et al. Proprioceptive loss and the perception, control and learning of arm movements in humans: evidence from sensory neuronopathy. Exp Brain Res. 2018;236:2137ā55.
Antfolk C, et al. Sensory feedback in upper limb prosthetics. Expert Rev Med Devices. 2013;10:45ā54.
Stephens-Fripp B, Alici G, Mutlu R. A review of non-invasive sensory feedback methods for transradial prosthetic hands. IEEE Access. 2018;3536:1ā1.
Stanley AA, Kuchenbecker KJ. Evaluation of tactile feedback methods for wrist rotation guidance. IEEE Trans Haptics. 2012;5:240ā51.
Witteveen HJB, Droog EA, Rietman JS, Veltink PH. Vibro- and electrotactile user feedback on hand opening for myoelectric forearm prostheses. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2012;59:2219ā26.
Cipriani C, Segil JL, Clemente F, Richard RF, Edin B. Humans can integrate feedback of discrete events in their sensorimotor control of a robotic hand. Exp Brain Res. 2014;232:3421ā9.
Krueger AR, Giannoni P, Shah V, Casadio M, Scheidt RA. Supplemental vibrotactile feedback control of stabilization and reaching actions of the arm using limb state and position error encodings. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2017;14:1ā23.
GuĆ©mann M, et al. Sensory substitution of elbow proprioception to improve myoelectric control of upper limb prosthesis: experiment on healthy subjects and amputees. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2022;19:1ā12.
Tchimino J, Dideriksen JL, Dosen S. EMG feedback outperforms force feedback in the presence of prosthesis control disturbance. Front Neurosci. 2022;16:1ā13.
Mirelman A, et al. Audio-biofeedback training for posture and balance in patients with Parkinsonās disease. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2011;8:35.
Shehata AW, Scheme EJ, Sensinger JW. Audible feedback improves internal model strength and performance of myoelectric prosthesis control. Sci Rep. 2018;8:8541.
Engels LF, Shehata AW, Scheme EJ, Sensinger JW, Cipriani C. When less is moreādiscrete tactile feedback dominates continuous audio biofeedback in the integrated percept while controlling a myoelectric prosthetic hand. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:1ā10.
Ortiz-Catalan M, HƄkansson B, BrƄnemark R, Hakansson B, Branemark R. An osseointegrated human-machine gateway for long-term sensory feedback and motor control of artificial limbs. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6:257re6.
Tan DW, et al. A neural interface provides long-term stable natural touch perception. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6:257ra138.
DāAnna E, et al. A closed-loop hand prosthesis with simultaneous intraneural tactile and position feedback. Sci Robot. 2019;4:eaau8892.
Markovic M, Schweisfurth MA, Engels LF, Farina D, Dosen S. Myocontrol is closed-loop control: Incidental feedback is sufficient for scaling the prosthesis force in routine grasping. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15:1ā11.
Marasco PD, et al. Illusory movement perception improves motor control for prosthetic hands. Sci Transl Med. 2018;10:eaao6990.
Markovic M, et al. The clinical relevance of advanced artificial feedback in the control of a multi-functional myoelectric prosthesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15:28.
Mastinu E, et al. Neural feedback strategies to improve grasping coordination in neuromusculoskeletal prostheses. Sci Rep. 2020;10:1ā15.
Cipriani C, Zaccone F, Micera S, Carrozza MC. On the shared control of an EMG-controlled prosthetic hand: analysis of user-prosthesis interaction. IEEE Trans Robot. 2008;24:170ā84.
Brown JD, et al. An exploration of grip force regulation with a low-impedance myoelectric prosthesis featuring referred haptic feedback. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12:1ā17.
Witteveen HJB, Rietman HS, Veltink PH. Vibrotactile grasping force and hand aperture feedback for myoelectric forearm prosthesis users. Prosthet Orthot Int. 2015;39:204ā12.
Christie BP, et al. Visual inputs and postural manipulations affect the location of somatosensory percepts elicited by electrical stimulation. Sci Rep. 2019;9:11699.
Ernst MO, Banks MS. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature. 2002;415:429ā33.
Hillis JM, Ernst MO, Banks MS, Landy MS. Combining sensory information: mandatory fusion within, but not between. Senses Science. 2002;298:1627ā30.
Earley EJ, Johnson RE, Hargrove LJ, Sensinger JW. Joint speed discrimination and augmentation for prosthesis feedback. Sci Rep. 2018;8:17752.
Earley EJ, Johnson RE, Sensinger JW, Hargrove LJ. Joint speed feedback improves myoelectric prosthesis adaptation after perturbed reaches in non amputees. Sci Rep. 2021;11:5158.
Clites TR, et al. Proprioception from a neurally controlled lower-extremity prosthesis. Sci Transl Med. 2018;10:eaap8373.
Pinzur MS. General principles of amputation surgery. In: Krajbich JI, Pinzur MS, Potter BK, Stevens PM, editors. Atlas of amputations and limb deficiencies surgical, prosthetic, and rehabilitation principles. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2018.
Schmidt RA, et al. Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol Rev. 1979;86:415.
Blustein D, Shehata A, Englehart K, Sensinger J. Conventional analysis of trial-by-trial adaptation is biased: empirical and theoretical support using a Bayesian estimator. PLOS Comput Biol. 2018;14: e1006501.
Blustein DH, Shehata AW, Kuylenstierna ES, Englehart KB, Sensinger JW. An analytical method reduces noise bias in motor adaptation analysis. Sci Rep. 2021;11:1ā12.
Johnson RE, Kording KP, Hargrove LJ, Sensinger JW. Adaptation to random and systematic errors: comparison of amputee and non-amputee control interfaces with varying levels of process noise. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:1ā19.
Burge J, Ernst MO, Banks MS. The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. J Vis. 2008;8(20):1ā19.
Huang VS, Haith A, Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW. Rethinking motor learning and savings in adaptation paradigms: model-free memory for successful actions combines with internal models. Neuron. 2011;70:787ā801.
Canaveral CA, Danion F, Berrigan F, Bernier P-M. Variance in exposed perturbations impairs retention of visuomotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2017;118:2745ā54.
Earley EJ, Kaveny KJ, Johnson RE, Hargrove LJ, Sensinger JW. Joint-based velocity feedback to virtual limb dynamic perturbations. In: IEEE . International Conference on Rehabilitation RoboticsāÆ: [proceedings] 1313ā1318 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1109/ICORR.2017.8009430.
Earley EJ, Johnson RE. Wrist speed feedback improves elbow compensation and reaching accuracy for myoelectric transradial prosthesis users in hybrid virtual reaching task. 2022;https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/NYRGE
Kutner MH. Applied linear statistical models. New York: McGraw-Hill Irwin; 2005.
Dosen S, Markovic M, Somer K, Graimann B, Farina D. EMG Biofeedback for online predictive control of grasping force in a myoelectric prosthesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12:55.
Schweisfurth MA, et al. Electrotactile EMG feedback improves the control of prosthesis grasping force. J Neural Eng. 2016;13:056010.
Williams HE, Chapman CS, Pilarski PM, Vette AH, Hebert JS. Myoelectric prosthesis users and non-disabled individuals wearing a simulated prosthesis exhibit similar compensatory movement strategies. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2021;18:1ā15.
Earley EJ, Hargrove LJ. Modeling expected reaching error and behaviors for motor adaptation. In: 2019 41st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) 1534ā1538 (IEEE, 2019). https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2019.8857562.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank everyone who agreed to participate in this study.
Funding
Funding for this research was provided by NSF-NRI 1317379 and NIH Grant T32 HD07418.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
EE prepared and conducted the experiments, performed statistical analysis, prepared the manuscript, and handled data availability. All authors planned the experiments. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Six subjects with transradial amputation participated in this study, which was approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board; all experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, and all subjects provided informed consent before starting the study.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any financial or non-financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. L.H. has ownership interest in Coapt LLC., a start-up company that sells myoelectric pattern recognition control systems. No Coapt products were used as part of this research.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Earley, E.J., Johnson, R.E., Sensinger, J.W. et al. Wrist speed feedback improves elbow compensation and reaching accuracy for myoelectric transradial prosthesis users in hybrid virtual reaching task. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 20, 9 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-023-01138-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-023-01138-3