Skip to main content

Neuromodulation of inhibitory control using phase-lagged transcranial alternating current stimulation

Abstract

Background

Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) is a prominent non-invasive brain stimulation method for modulating neural oscillations and enhancing human cognitive function. This study aimed to investigate the effects of individualized theta tACS delivered in-phase and out-of-phase between the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) during inhibitory control performance.

Methods

The participants engaged in a Stroop task with phase-lagged theta tACS over individually optimized high-density electrode montages targeting the dACC and lDLPFC. We analyzed task performance, event-related potentials, and prestimulus electroencephalographic theta and alpha power.

Results

We observed significantly reduced reaction times following out-of-phase tACS, accompanied by reduced frontocentral N1 and N2 amplitudes, enhanced parieto-occipital P1 amplitudes, and pronounced frontocentral late sustained potentials. Out-of-phase stimulation also resulted in significantly higher prestimulus frontocentral theta and alpha activity.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that out-of-phase theta tACS potently modulates top-down inhibitory control, supporting the feasibility of phase-lagged tACS to enhance inhibitory control performance.

Background

Situations arise daily in which various features of our surroundings compete for attention and distract us from tasks. Cognitive control is a vital component of our daily lives, guiding our actions by promoting relevant information and suppressing irrelevant details or habitual behaviors. As cognitive control plays an important role in goal-directed behavior and is essential to effective functioning [1], techniques aimed at improving or enhancing cognitive control can provide valuable insight into and deepen our understanding of its underlying mechanisms and neural correlates. Although several studies have attempted to shed light on this topic [2,3,4,5], the neurophysiological rationales underlying these varying results remain unclear. This study aimed to provide neuromodulatory insight into the neurodynamics of cognitive control.

The Stroop task [6] is a well-established paradigm wherein cognitive inhibitory control distinguishes between intentional and automatic behaviors. For instance, in incongruent conditions, when word and text colors are conflicting, the color naming response time is longer than word reading, and color naming errors are more prevalent. This behavioral trend, referred to as the Stroop effect, implies that suppression of the automatic word-reading process is more challenging and requires inhibitory control for the color naming response [6, 7]. Functional neuroimaging studies uncovered numerous frontal brain regions that support cognitive control in the Stroop task, notably the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) [8,9,10,11,12]. The lDLPFC maintains a representation of task-related demands, regulates visual processes, and directs attention to relevant aspects of stimuli [9, 13,14,15]. Moreover, it plays a central role in exerting control over behavior, with extensive connections to the sensory and motor areas [1, 16]. The dACC is also associated with attentional processes, including error detection, conflict monitoring, and performance evaluation [17,18,19]. Activity in this region closely mirrors the level of control recruited in conflict scenarios and has a strong functional relationship with the lateral prefrontal cortex [20,21,22].

The most basic computational model explaining control processes in the Stroop task uses two competing pathways: automatic (encoding a word feature) and control-demanding (encoding a color feature) processes [23]. This model can be expanded to incorporate a conflict-monitoring unit represented by the dACC, which signals the DLPFC-mediated cognitive control required to facilitate processing in the task-relevant pathway [11, 18]. Recent models further explored and tested the temporal dynamics between the two regions [24,25,26,27]. For instance, the “cascade of control” model [27] proposes that control mechanisms intervene at various task performance stages to resolve the Stroop conflict effect (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

“Cascade of control” model of the Stroop effect (adapted from [27]). DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

The interaction between the lDLPFC and the dACC can also be investigated electrophysiologically. The two regions exhibit stronger connectivity in conflict situations such as the incongruent condition in the Stroop task, particularly in the theta frequency band (4–8 Hz) [26, 28,29,30]. Furthermore, increased theta power has been observed in the midfrontal regions after events that require greater control [31,32,33]. These theta dynamics play a crucial role in coordinating diverse brain regions and orchestrating the temporal flow of midfrontal neuronal processes, thus enabling executive conflict control functions [30, 33]. Studies conducted in conjunction with functional imaging methods or involving source reconstruction suggested that the medial prefrontal cortex generates this oscillatory activity [28, 31, 34, 35]. This activity is often accompanied by signature event-related potential (ERP) components such as N2 and late sustained potentials (LSP) [36, 37].

On the other hand, non-invasive neuromodulation of cognitive control processes may impact cognitive performance. Previous studies have used various non-invasive brain stimulation methodologies such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) [38,39,40,41]. Several studies have explored the causal influence of specific brain oscillations on cognitive function [42,43,44]. By employing these approaches and manipulating specific stimulation parameters, researchers have successfully demonstrated the intentional modulation of brain activity as well as improved performance in cognitive tasks, such as working memory performance [45,46,47,48] or inhibitory control [49, 50]. For example, neuronal communication depends on the coherent oscillation of activated neuronal groups, which enables effective interactions through synchronized communication windows and supports cognitive flexibility [51]. By using tACS to synchronize intrinsic neuronal oscillations to the applied stimulation phase, brain rhythms can be effectively entrained through phase specificity [52, 53]. Phase synchronization is an essential neuronal mechanism that manages intrinsic communication between distinct nodes to improve cognitive functions such as executive skills, attention, and context processing [54,55,56]. Accordingly, in-phase tACS seeks to enhance synchronization and coordination between brain regions to improve specific cognitive functions, whereas out-of-phase tACS aims to introduce interference or desynchronization to modulate cognitive processes [57,58,59,60,61]. Nevertheless, the specific type of phase lag that can effectively enhance task performance and the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms remain unclear.

Thus, here we designed a paradigm to investigate whether theta-frequency tACS administered with different relative phase lags between the dACC and the lDLPFC affects cognitive control performance. It has been reported that the influence of lDLPFC on Stroop interference is mediated by later dACC activity [25], consistent with the temporal course hypothesis posited by the “cascade of control” model [27]. The different phase lags simulate temporally delayed stimulation between the lDLPFC and the dACC. We used the color-word Stroop task [6] to examine the effects of tACS on inhibitory function and executive (top-down) control. Individual theta peak frequencies were employed, and the dACC and lDLPFC were stimulated in-phase (0° relative phase lag) or out-of-phase (180° relative phase lag). We hypothesized that out-of-phase (phase-lagged) tACS would effectively modulate inhibitory task performance. Owing to highly contaminated noise in the EEG data that is created by simultaneous tACS currents, the EEG data were obtained during the Stroop task after each tACS session was completed.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-four healthy volunteers (mean age, 23.67 ± 0.53 years; 13 men, 11 women) participated in this study. All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of the participants reported having a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, and no color blindness was determined using the Ishihara color test. All participants were free of contraindications to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanning. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Korea University Institutional Review Board (KUIRB-2021-0209-08).

Task paradigm and experimental procedure

We used the color-word Stroop task [6] to investigate inhibitory control functions (Fig. 2A). The Stroop task is suitable for studying neural activity related to cognitive control since it requires attentional allocation and inhibitory processes with conflicting features. The task stimuli comprised congruent, neutral, and incongruent conditions. The congruent and incongruent stimuli were color words (“Red” and “Green”) written in congruent or incongruent colors, while neutral stimuli were meaningless streams of letters (“XXX”). The items were presented randomly and equally to each participant using presentation software (E-prime 3.0 Professional, Psychology Software Tools, USA). The subtended visual angle for each item was set at 5°. The participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible by pressing a button with their right or left index finger indicating the color of the presented letter stimulus. The response hands were counterbalanced across the participants. As depicted in Fig. 2A, a fixation cross was presented at the center of the screen for 1500 ms, followed by presentation of the stimulus for 1500 ms. During this time, the participants’ responses were recorded. For an additional 1000 ms, visual feedback in the form of “Correct,” “Incorrect,” or “No Response” was displayed on the monitor to motivate the participants and encourage improved performance of the task. Each participant completed three task sessions, each comprising 45 trials per congruency condition for a total of 135 trials per task session. The initial task session was used to determine the individual theta peak frequencies. The individual theta frequencies for each participant were determined as the dominant theta peak frequency during the Stroop task performance. Based on the EEG data from the first experimental session without tACS treatment (Fig. 2B), the theta frequencies (4–8 Hz) for each participant were individually determined and were administered as personalized theta-frequency sinusoidal tACS signals. This approach is based on the close relationship between the midfrontal theta frequency band during conflict situations and the central executive function, as demonstrated in previous research [31, 33]. Before the main tACS experiment was performed, the midfrontal theta peak frequencies were individually determined using the fast Fourier transform (without padding) of artifact-free EEG trials. The frequency (ranging from 4 to 8 Hz) was selected when its maximum power at the midfrontal electrodes Fz, F1, and F2 was detected within the time window between stimulus onset and 1 s poststimulus during the no-tACS Stroop task (mean theta peak frequency: 5.79 ± 1.61 Hz).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Experimental procedure and the Stroop task. (A) The Stroop task paradigm. (B) Time flow of the experimental sessions. Each stimulation session was followed by the task session. The order of the stimulation sessions (in-phase/out-of-phase or out-of-phase/in-phase) was counterbalanced across the participants. tACS, transcranial alternating current stimulation

Each experiment consisted of five 9-min-long sessions, between which short breaks were given (Fig. 2B). We conducted two stimulation sessions, during which tACS was applied for 9 min with a 0° (in-phase) or 180° (out-of-phase) phase difference between the lDLPFC and dACC (Fig. 3). The order of the stimulation sessions was counterbalanced across the participants. Each stimulation session was followed by the performance of a Stroop task. The participants were debriefed immediately after the last task session, during which they indicated their subjective perception (or any uncomfortable experience, including retinal phosphenes) of the stimulation. Eventually, 21 of the 24 participants did not report irritation induced by the tACS treatment. One individual experienced dizziness, another reported sore eyes, and the remaining participant reported a migraine-like feeling at times.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Stimulation protocol for in-phase and out-of-phase tACS. (A) In-phase stimulation waveforms to the lDLPFC (red solid line) and dACC (blue dashed line). (B) Out-of-phase stimulation waveforms to the lDLPFC (solid red line) and dACC (dashed blue line). (C) A sample simulation of the tACS-induced electric field at the lDLPFC and dACC. The unit |V/m| denotes the normalized strength of the induced electric field. (D) The simulated electric intensity (V/m) of each phase bin (eight bins for \(2{\pi }\)—that is, by a step of \(\frac{2{\pi }}{8}\)) is plotted in the lDLPFC (two upper plots in red) and in the dACC (two lower plots in blue; a left panel for the 0°-phase-lag and a right panel for the 180°-phase-lag tACS condition). Note that the phase of the lDLPFC stimulus advanced that of the dACC stimulus by approximately 180° (vertical green dashed lines indicate peak phases of the lDLPFC and dACC). dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; lDLPFC, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

Target localization using fMRI

Before the main experiment (with a minimum interval of two days preceding the main experiment), we performed functional MRI (fMRI) to individually localize the task-relevant brain regions that were activated during the Stroop task and to optimize the stimulation electrode placement. We used the initial fMRI data during the Stroop task without tACS to individually identify the target regions, specifically the lDLPFC and dACC. Throughout the Stroop task, whole-brain images were acquired using a 32-channel head coil within a 3T MAGNETOM Trio Tim Syngo scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany). A total of 270 blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) fMRI image volumes were acquired using an interleaved T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR), 2000 ms; echo time (TE), 30 ms; flip angle (FA), 90°; multi-band acceleration factor, 3; acquisition matrix, 96 × 96; field of view, 192 × 192 mm2; in-plane voxel size, 2 × 2 × 2 mm3; and no slice gap. High-resolution structural scans of three-dimensional anatomical magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo images were obtained for each subject after the fMRI data collection (TR, 2.3 s; TE, 2.13 ms; inversion time, 0.9 s; FA, 9°; acquisition matrix, 256 × 256; in-plane voxel size, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3; 224 sagittal slices).

Following the preprocessing of BOLD images through the standard task-based fMRI pipeline (slice-timing correction, motion correction, co-registration, grey/white matter segmentation, normalization, and spatial smoothing using a 6-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel) with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12) in MATLAB software (R2021a; MathWorks, USA), we analyzed whole-brain activity for each trial type (congruent, incongruent, and neutral) relative to a fixation block for each participant. Subject-specific optimized coordinates of the stimulated target regions, specifically the lDLPFC and the dACC, were co-registered on individual T1 images to ensure a spatially accurate stimulation. A sample montage for a single individual is shown in Fig. 4A. Generally, an input-return module configuration is used to coordinate the stimulation and return channels (i.e., return electrodes are arranged around the stimulation electrode). The currents of all stimulation and return channels were set to maintain the total amount of current at 0.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Target-region identification and electrode-placement montage. (A) An example of target-region identification using individual functional magnetic resonance imaging data. (B) A sample montage of the optimized electrode placements used for high-definition transcranial alternating current stimulation. The stimulation input electrode for each region of interest (highlighted in green) is marked in red, and the three return electrodes are marked in blue. The montages of all participants are provided in Supplementary Figure S1. dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; lDLPFC, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

EEG acquisition and tACS stimulation

Before the electroencephalography (EEG) experiment, we conducted an fMRI test to localize the brain regions activated during the Stroop task and to optimize the stimulation electrode placement. We used fMRI data from each participant to identify the target regions, specifically the lDLPFC and the dACC. The electrode placement was then planned to maximize the tACS-induced electric field intensity in the target regions. Optimization of electrode placement was performed using SimNIBS (ver. 3.2.6, DRCMR & DTU, Denmark) [62] and tES LAB (ver. 3.0, NeuroPhet, Seoul, Korea). To ensure precise, spatially accurate stimulation, participant-specific optimized coordinates of the target regions for stimulation were calculated based on individual T1 images that were obtained prior to the main experiment. Electrode placements were selected to optimize the strength of the electric field at the target locations. The optimal stimulation power was determined based on the attainment of maximum intensity at both target regions (i.e., the lDLPFC and dACC) when manipulating the combination of input/return electrode positions around the scalp of the target regions using the individual sensation thresholds of the input current. For each target region, we selected one stimulation electrode (marked in red) and three return electrodes (marked in blue; Fig. 4B).

High-definition tACS was administered using a 65-channel high-definition transcranial electrical stimulation (HD-tES) device (M×N 65 HD-tES; Soterix Medical Inc., USA), with individually customized frequency and intensity for each participant. Ag/AgCl sintered ring electrodes (HD-Electrode, Soterix Medical Inc., USA; surface area: 1.13 cm2) were used for stimulation and affixed to the scalp using an electrolyte medium (HD-GEL, Soterix Medical Inc., USA) in an EEG cap (actiCAP, Brain Products GmbH, Germany). Prior to the stimulation experiment, the impedances of the stimulation electrodes were maintained below 30 kΩ. Regarding the individually applied stimulation amplitude, the mean amplitude was 0.86 ± 0.33 mA. Since the lDLPFC and dACC exhibit stronger connectivity in conflict situations, such as the incongruent condition in the Stroop task, particularly in the EEG theta band [26, 28,29,30], and the neuromodulatory effect of resonating the brain oscillations responsible for Stroop task performance would be maximized when the stimulus wave (i.e., tACS) resembles the target wave (i.e., human brain wave) as closely as possible, the theta frequency was used for the tACS frequency in the present study. The theta frequencies (4–8 Hz) for each participant were individually determined based on the first experimental session and administered as personalized theta-frequency sinusoidal waves. This approach is based on the close relationship between the midfrontal theta frequency band during conflict situations and central executive function [31, 33]. We adjusted the stimulation intensity individually for each participant in a stepwise manner to ensure that it was below the individual sensation threshold and that the total stimulation intensity did not exceed 1.5 mA.

A simulation program (tES LAB software, ver. 3.0, NeuroPhet, Korea) was used to examine whether the stimulation signals matched the intended phase lag (0° or 180°) before the main study. The mean simulation electric-field intensity was 0.11 ± 0.02 V/m at the activated cortical region. As shown in Fig. 3C, the simulation results demonstrated that the activation regions were well aligned with the intended target regions. Moreover, we investigated whether the phase difference between the lDLPFC and the dACC stimulation signals exactly matched the intended phase lag (0° or 180°; Fig. 3D).

EEG signals were recorded using a BrainAmp DC amplifier (Brain Products, Germany) with 64 Ag/AgCl electrodes, according to the international 10–10 system. A reference electrode was placed at the tip of the nose and the AFz electrode was used as the ground electrode. Electrode input impedances were kept below 10 kΩ prior to the data acquisition. The sampling rate was set at 500 Hz.

Data analysis

We analyzed both reaction times and accuracy as behavioral measures of task performance. Each participant’s reaction times were fitted to a gamma distribution and collected within a 95% confidence interval [63]. For the subsequent EEG analyses, only correct response trials were selected. Preprocessing of the electrophysiological data was performed offline using the EEGLAB toolbox [64]. We applied a 0.5-Hz high-pass filter and a 60-Hz notch filter to the raw EEG signals. Eye and muscle artifacts were automatically identified with an 80% threshold (probability, 0.8) and removed from the data using independent component analysis and ICLabel, an automated independent component classifier [65, 66]. The EEG data were segmented from the 1500 ms prestimulus to the 2500 ms poststimulus, with a total of 4000 ms for each epoch. After epoching the EEG data, demeaning and detrending were performed. Subsequent analyses were performed using MATLAB software (R2022b; MathWorks, USA).

To investigate the ERP components, we further filtered the EEG signals from 1 to 30 Hz. The averaged signals were baseline-corrected using a 200-ms time window before stimulus presentation. We inspected the tACS-mediated ERP alterations during the Stroop task. Time ranges were determined based on previous ERP studies on cognitive control [36, 37] and adjusted according to the grand averages with individual variations, while considering the time windows of adjacent ERP components. N1, P1, and N2 were designated as early ERP components, whereas LSP was considered a late ERP component. Electrodes for the ERP analyses were selected based on the regions of the brain in which the activity was most prominent. For the frontocentral N1 component, negative peak amplitudes were detected within a time window of 30–130 ms for the FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz, C1, C2, CPz, CP1, and CP2 electrodes. For the parieto-occipital P1 component, positive peak amplitudes were detected within a time window of 20–120 ms for the O1, O2, PO7, and PO8 electrodes. For the frontocentral N2 component, negative peak amplitudes were detected within a time window of 150–250 ms for the Fz, F1, F2, FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz, C1, and C2 electrodes. For the frontocentral LSP, the mean amplitudes were detected within a time window of 550–800 ms for the Fz, F1, F2, FCz, FC1, FC2, Cz, C1, and C2 electrodes.

To investigate EEG oscillatory activity, we performed a complex Morlet wavelet convolution of the EEG signals. We employed a wavelet family with a constant ratio of seven cycles [67] and frequencies of 1–15 Hz. A wavelet transform was conducted for each trial, and the absolute values of the resulting transforms were averaged. This measure of signal amplitude in single trials reflects the total activity for a certain frequency range. As prestimulus theta and alpha activity are known indicators of top-down preparation [68, 69], we investigated theta (4–8 Hz) and alpha (8–13 Hz) power before the stimulus presentation. Electrodes for the spectral analyses were selected based on the task-relevant topographic distribution, and the analytic time windows were selected based on the smearing effects of the wavelet analysis and the effective numbers of wavelengths of the analytic frequencies. For the prestimulus frontocentral theta activity, theta power was computed in an interval of 200–500 ms before stimulus onset and averaged across the Fz, F1, F2, and FCz electrodes. For prestimulus frontocentral and parieto-occipital alpha activity, alpha power was computed in a time window of 100–400 ms before stimulus onset and averaged across the Fz, F1, F2, and FCz, as well as across the POz, PO3, and PO4 electrodes, respectively. The Shapiro–Wilk test revealed that the data was not normally distributed. Therefore, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test, a non-parametric approach, was employed in this study. Behavioral data, ERP components, and EEG spectral power were compared between the in-phase and out-of-phase stimulation conditions within each congruency condition. A statistical power analysis [70] for the Wilcoxon signed-rank test estimated the minimum number of samples, ≥ 15 (24 participants in the present study), with an effective size = 0.8, statistical power (1-𝛽) = 0.8, and ɑ = 0.05.

Results

Behavioral data

In the incongruent condition, out-of-phase stimulation exhibited significantly reduced reaction times compared to in-phase stimulation (Z = 2.49, p < 0.05; in-phase, 603.26 ms; out-of-phase, 549.17 ms; Fig. 5A). The differences in reaction times between in-phase and out-of-phase tACS in the congruent (Z = 0.66, n.s.) and neutral (Z = 0.63, n.s.) conditions were not statistically significant. There were no significant differences between the stimulation conditions in terms of task performance accuracy for the congruent (Z = 1.57, n.s.), neutral (Z = − 1.11, n.s.), and incongruent (Z = 1.25, n.s.) conditions (Fig. 5B).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Phase-dependent tACS-mediated changes in reaction times and accuracies. (A) Reaction times following in-phase (red) and out-of-phase (blue) stimulation. (B) Task performance accuracies following in-phase (red) and out-of-phase (blue) stimulation. In the box plots, boxes are drawn from the first to the third quartile. The horizontal lines within boxes denote the median, and the whiskers extend from each quartile to the minimum or maximum with excluded outliers marked as small crosses. The asterisk represents statistical significance (*p < 0.05)

ERP components

For the N1 component, we found significantly reduced N1 amplitudes in the out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation condition in the incongruent (Z = − 2.31, p < 0.05; in-phase, − 4.35 µV; out-of-phase, − 3.11 µV) and neutral (Z = − 2.57, p < 0.05; in-phase, − 4.42 µV; out-of-phase, − 3.18 µV) conditions. The differences in the congruent condition were not statistically significant (Z = − 0.51, n.s.). Regarding the parieto-occipital P1 component, we observed significantly enhanced P1 amplitudes in the out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation condition in the incongruent condition (Z = − 2.11, p < 0.05; in-phase, 2.49 µV; out-of-phase, 3.97 µV). The differences in P1 amplitudes between them in the congruent (Z = − 0.94, n.s.) and neutral (Z = − 1.94, n.s.) conditions were not statistically significant (Fig. 6).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Phase-dependent tACS-mediated topographical maps and time courses of N1 and P1 components in the incongruent condition. (A) The upper panel illustrates the grand-averaged topographical distributions for the N1 component (at 90 ms poststimulus). The lower panel shows the grand-averaged ERP time courses for in-phase (orange dotted line) and out-of-phase (blue solid line) stimulation at electrode Cz. (B) The upper panel illustrates the grand-averaged topographical distributions for the P1 component (at 100 ms poststimulus). The lower panel shows the grand-averaged ERP time courses for in-phase (orange dotted line) and out-of-phase (blue solid line) stimulation at the O1 electrode. Topographies are displayed in the order of in-phase (left) and out-of-phase (right) tACS. The view of the topography is from the vertex perspective with the nose at the top of the image. For ERP time courses, time zero indicates stimulus onset. The error bands indicate the standard errors of the mean, and the asterisks represent statistical significance (*p < 0.05)

In addition, significantly reduced frontocentral N2 amplitudes were detected in the out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation condition in the incongruent condition (Z = − 2.31, p < 0.05; in-phase, − 3.02 µV; out-of-phase, − 1.37 µV). The differences in the N2 amplitudes between them in the congruent (Z = 0.63, n.s.) and neutral (Z = − 1.20, n.s.) conditions did not reach statistical significance. Finally, for the frontocentral LSP, the mean LSP amplitudes were significantly higher in the out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation condition in the incongruent condition (Z = − 1.97, p < 0.05; in-phase, 2.79 µV; out-of-phase, 4.31 µV). The differences between them in the congruent (Z = − 1.86, n.s.) and neutral (Z = − 1.20, n.s.) conditions were not statistically significant (Fig. 7).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Phase-dependent tACS-mediated topographical maps and time courses of N2 and LSP in the incongruent condition. The upper panel illustrates the grand-averaged topographical distributions for the (A) N2 (at 225 ms poststimulus) and (B) LSP (averaged over 550 to 800 ms poststimulus). The lower panel shows the grand-averaged ERP time courses for the in-phase (orange dotted line) and out-of-phase (blue solid line) stimulation at the electrode FCz. Topographies are displayed in the order of in-phase (left) and out-of-phase (right) tACS. The view of the topography is from the vertex, with the nose at the top of the image. For ERP time courses, time zero indicates stimulus onset. The error bands indicate the standard errors of the mean, and the asterisks represent statistical significance (*p < 0.05)

EEG spectral power

Regarding the prestimulus frontocentral theta activity, significantly enhanced theta power was observed in the out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation condition in the congruent (Z = − 2.43, p < 0.05; in-phase, 2.84 µV2; out-of-phase, 3.99 µV2) and incongruent (Z = − 2.51, p < 0.05; in-phase, 2.75 µV2; out-of-phase, 3.58 µV2) conditions. The differences between the two stimulation conditions in the neutral condition did not reach statistical significance (Z = − 1.69, n.s.). Regarding prestimulus alpha activity in the parieto-occipital region, there were no significant differences between the two stimulation conditions in the congruent (Z = 0.20, n.s.), neutral (Z = 0.11, n.s.), and incongruent (Z = − 0.14, n.s.) conditions. However, significantly enhanced prestimulus alpha power was detected over the frontocentral regions in the out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation condition in the congruent (Z = − 2.23, p < 0.05; in-phase, 2.01 µV2; out-of-phase, 2.47 µV2) and incongruent (Z = − 2.23, p < 0.05; in-phase, 2.11 µV2; out-of-phase, 2.73 µV2) conditions (Fig. 8). The differences between the two stimulation conditions in the neutral condition (Z = − 1.60, n.s.) were not statistically significant. Since the spectral power showed significant differences between in-phase and out-of-phase tACS treatments, particularly in the theta and alpha bands (Fig. 8C), these observations were not simply due to the 1/f-like power spectral distribution of the EEG data [71].

Fig. 8
figure 8

Phase-dependent tACS-mediated topographical maps of prestimulus theta and alpha power in the incongruent condition. (A) The topographical maps show the grand-averaged prestimulus theta (500 to 200 ms prestimulus) and alpha (400 to 100 ms prestimulus) power distributions given in the order of in-phase (left) and out-of-phase (right) stimulation. The view of the topography is from the vertex perspective with the nose at the top of the image. (B) The time-frequency plots represent the spectral power of total activity at the electrode Fz. Time 0 indicates stimulus onset. The color bar indicates the power (µV2). (C) Comparison of prestimulus spectral power (µV2; in the frontocentral region averaged across Fz, F1, F2, and FCz, from 400 to 200 ms prestimulus) between in-phase (red bars) and out-of-phase (blue bars) tACS treatments across the delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (30–50 Hz) bands. The error bars indicate the standard errors of the mean, and the asterisks represent statistical significance (*p < 0.05). Note the phase-dependent tACS-mediated significant differences particularly in theta and alpha bands

Discussion

Using phase-lagged tACS between two task-relevant brain regions, we observed improved behavioral performance and corresponding neurophysiological signatures during the inhibitory control task. Although previous non-invasive brain stimulation studies investigated the effects of tACS on inhibitory control by manipulating stimulation parameters, the utilization of temporal (or phasic) relationships between lDLPFC and dACC activity during inhibitory control is often overlooked. In the present study, out-of-phase stimulation between the lDLPFC and dACC yielded significantly faster task performance, significantly reduced N1 and N2 amplitudes, and significantly enhanced P1 and LSP amplitudes than in-phase stimulation, particularly in the incongruent condition. Additionally, out-of-phase stimulation resulted in significantly pronounced prestimulus frontocentral theta and alpha activity. Overall, our results demonstrate that phase-lagged tACS across task-relevant brain regions could effectively modulate the behavioral and neurophysiological aspects of inhibitory control.

Regarding the tACS-mediated alterations in ERP components, frontocentral N1 deflection exhibited reduced negative peak amplitudes following out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation. N1 amplitude enhancement has been reported in relation to attentional facilitation directed toward the initial sensory processing of letter-component visual features such as line orientation and curvature [72,73,74]. Thus, the relatively enhanced N1 amplitude mediated by in-phase tACS promoted a more automatic but task-irrelevant word-reading process, consequently interrupting color-perceiving task performance. In this respect, out-of-phase stimulation might induce relatively less conflict between color and letter dimensions, facilitating the task-relevant color-perception process. A similar trend observed in the neutral condition, in which word reading was not prioritized, further supports this interpretation. The parieto-occipital P1 peaks were more prominent after out-of-phase than in-phase stimulation. This may reflect the facilitation of feature-specific top-down processing, specifically a boost in selective attention to color-based features [15, 75,76,77,78]. In effect, early bias toward task-relevant color processing was more pronounced in the out-of-phase stimulation sessions.

The subsequent frontocentral N2 component exhibited reduced peak amplitudes during out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation. With sources in the medial prefrontal regions, this reduction may indicate an out-of-phase tACS-mediated decrease in conflict activation or a lower demand to select task-relevant information before responding [34, 35, 79, 80]. This observation aligns with previous results showing that improved cognitive control is indexed by reduced dACC activation, quicker response times, and diminished N2 peak amplitudes [81,82,83]. Therefore, we can infer that top-down control functions were strengthened following out-of-phase stimulation, resulting in a more effective inhibition of distractors and a stronger bias toward task-relevant information. Finally, for the frontocentral LSP, we noted enhanced mean amplitudes during out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation. This enhancement likely signifies the reinforced involvement of top-down executive processes and conflict resolution with sources in the lateral frontal cortices [28, 84, 85]. It is plausible that out-of-phase stimulation leads to an augmentation of top-down control, resulting in heightened selectivity toward task-relevant features and improved suppression of irrelevant information.

The EEG spectral analysis consistently supported the neuromodulatory effect of out-of-phase tACS on inhibitory control. Regarding EEG theta activity, out-of-phase tACS yielded significantly enhanced prestimulus theta power around the frontocentral region. This tACS-mediated augmentation likely indicates heightened anticipation and pre-activation of cognitive control for the impending task [68, 86,87,88,89,90,91,92]. Although previous studies linking theta power increase to preparation for subsequent conflict often presented cues before stimulus onset [87, 89,90,91], our experimental instructions required the participants to maintain the task strategy (i.e., recognizing colors of colored letters) throughout the task sessions. Furthermore, because the intertrial intervals in the present study were constant, there is likely an endogenously generated temporal anticipation of the upcoming trial [86, 92]. Taken together, out-of-phase tACS-mediated enhancement of top-down preparation may facilitate quicker adjustments when conflicts are encountered in a subsequent task interval, resulting in reduced reaction times. Accordingly, we also observed significantly prominent prestimulus alpha activity in the frontocentral region for out-of-phase versus in-phase stimulation. This increased prestimulus alpha activity implies improved inhibitory suppression of task-irrelevant processing associated with word reading, reflecting reinforced top-down regulatory control [69, 93,94,95,96]. Accordingly, the precise allocation of attention to task-relevant features appears to be facilitated by out-of-phase stimulation. Our ERP results also support this interpretation, demonstrating that preparatory attention influences early neural responses to stimulus features, consistent with the findings of previous studies [76, 95, 97,98,99].

Our neuromodulatory findings were consistent with those of previous neurodynamic studies on inhibitory control. For example, the control model [27] posits a temporal sequence in interference processing during a Stroop task. According to this model, DLPFC regions implement early top-down control by assigning greater importance to task-relevant sensory processing. Subsequently, cingulate regions select the appropriate information required to generate a response and provide feedback to the relevant areas [24, 25, 27]. The dual-network view of the attentional system [100] proposes a similar theory in which distinct networks collaborate in implementing top-down control. Specifically, the cingulo-opercular control system, encompassing the dACC, and the frontoparietal system, comprising the DLPFC, interact with unique roles [12, 100, 101]. Within this framework, a temporal flow exists and the interactions between the subregions of each network influence the task performance [101].

However, the present study has several limitations. First, owing to current technical limitations, the scalp-based tACS device was unable to effectively stimulate deep brain structures such as the dACC (Fig. 3C). Moreover, individual sensation thresholds of the input current contributed to further limitations in the stimulation power to the target areas. Because the mean simulation electric field (0.11 V/m) was below the minimum of 0.2 V/m that is required to modulate neurons in awake and behaving mammals [102, 103], a higher stimulation intensity would have improved the observed neuromodulatory effect. A recent neuromodulatory approach using temporal interference has demonstrated a non-invasive method for selectively stimulating deep brain structures [104, 105], which may provide further corroborating evidence for future studies. Second, the EEG theta frequency should be calculated under the no-tACS condition for the subsequent tACS resonating frequency, and a long experimental time (when both no-tACS and sham conditions are included) could induce fatigue in participants, leading to poor data quality. To consider this trade-off, the present study employed only the no-tACS condition, excluding the tACS-sham condition. Therefore, the present study focused on the tACS phase-dependent modulatory effect of in-phase (0°) and out-of-phase (180°) lags across the lDLPFC and dACC on subsequent behavioral and neurodynamic changes. Third, the in-phase and out-of-phase lag conditions were applied in a serial manner with a time gap of at least 10 min (Fig. 2B). Although there was a counter-balanced arrangement across the participants to cancel out the tACS after-effects, any possible tACS after-effects [106, 107] should be carefully considered when interpreting our findings.

Conclusions

Our findings suggested that out-of-phase stimulation facilitated inhibitory control processing using optimally phase-lagged stimulation signals across task-relevant brain regions (i.e., the lDLPFC and dACC), thereby strengthening temporal neurodynamics across them. This neuromodulatory augmentation may effectively mitigate the Stroop interference effect and boost the inhibitory control processes. Notably, this effect remained unaffected by the speed-accuracy trade-off, resulting in improved reaction times without compromising task performance accuracy. It is probable that fine-tuning the phase lags of the tACS signals between the two task-relevant brain regions would provide further optimized neuromodulation. Additionally, spatially accurate neuroimaging approaches such as fMRI may reveal detailed tACS-mediated neurodynamics observed in task-relevant subcortical structures for inhibitory control. In summary, our observations provide promising evidence that out-of-phase tACS between the lDLPFC and the dACC effectively modulates selective attention and prestimulus top-down regulation, thereby facilitating inhibitory control performance. Our findings suggest that at least some of the variability in non-invasive brain stimulation effects may be attributed to temporal phase relationships across task-relevant brain regions, suggesting that this information should be considered in neuromodulatory paradigm designs.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Abbreviations

BOLD:

blood oxygenation level–dependent

dACC:

dorsal anterior cingulate cortex

EEG:

electroencephalography

ERP:

event-related potential

FA:

flip angle

fMRI:

functional magnetic resonance imaging

lDLPFC:

left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

LSP:

late sustained potentials

MRI:

magnetic resonance imaging

tACS:

transcranial alternating current stimulation

TE:

echo time

TR:

repetition time

References

  1. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Fusco G, Scandola M, Feurra M, Pavone EF, Rossi S, Aglioti SM. Midfrontal theta transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates behavioural adjustment after error execution. Eur J Neurosci. 2018;48(10):3159–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lehr A, Henneberg N, Nigam T, Paulus W, Antal A. Modulation of conflict processing by theta-range tacs over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neural Plast. 2019;2019:6747049.

  4. Klirova M, Vorackova V, Horacek J, Mohr P, Jonas J, Dudysova DU, Kostylkova L, Fayette D, Krejcova L, Baumann S, et al. Modulating inhibitory control processes using individualized high definition theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (HD theta-tACS) of the anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Front Syst Neurosci. 2021;15:611507.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Jones KT, Johnson EL, Gazzaley A, Zanto TP. Structural and functional network mechanisms of rescuing cognitive control in aging. NeuroImage. 2022;262:119547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull. 1991;109(2):163–203.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. MacDonald AW 3rd, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2000;288(5472):1835–8.

  9. Banich MT, Milham MP, Jacobson BL, Webb A, Wszalek T, Cohen NJ, Kramer AF. Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: insights from fMRI examinations of the stroop task. Prog Brain Res. 2001;134:459–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J. Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(1):1–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Krug MK, Carter CS. Conflict Control Loop Theory of Cognitive Control. In: The Neuroscience of Attention: Attentional Control and Selection Edited by Mangun GR: Oxford University Press; 2012:0.

  12. Menon V, D’Esposito M. The role of PFC networks in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47(1):90–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Banich MT, Milham MP, Atchley RA, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Wszalek T, Kramer AF, Liang Z, Barad V, Gullett D, et al. Prefrontal regions play a predominant role in imposing an attentional ‘set’: evidence from fMRI. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2000;10(1–2):1–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Egner T, Hirsch J. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(12):1784–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Polk TA, Drake RM, Jonides JJ, Smith MR, Smith EE. Attention enhances the neural processing of relevant features and suppresses the processing of irrelevant features in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the stroop task. J Neurosci. 2008;28(51):13786–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Vanderhasselt MA, De Raedt R, Baeken C. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and stroop performance: tackling the lateralization. Psychon Bull Rev. 2009;16(3):609–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(6):215–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108(3):624–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron. 2013;79(2):217–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303(5660):1023–6.

  21. Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(12):539–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Botvinick MM. Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(4):356–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Cohen JD, Dunbar K, McClelland JL. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychol Rev. 1990;97(3):332–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Banich MT. Executive function: the search for an integrated account. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18(2):89–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Silton RL, Heller W, Towers DN, Engels AS, Spielberg JM, Edgar JC, Sass SM, Stewart JL, Sutton BP, Banich MT, et al. The time course of activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex during top-down attentional control. NeuroImage. 2010;50(3):1292–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Oehrn CR, Hanslmayr S, Fell J, Deuker L, Kremers NA, Do Lam AT, Elger CE, Axmacher N. Neural communication patterns underlying conflict detection, resolution, and adaptation. J Neurosci. 2014;34(31):10438–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Banich MT. The Stroop Effect occurs at multiple points along a cascade of control: evidence from cognitive neuroscience approaches. Front Psychol. 2019;10:2164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Hanslmayr S, Pastotter B, Bauml KH, Gruber S, Wimber M, Klimesch W. The electrophysiological dynamics of interference during the stroop task. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20(2):215–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cohen MX, Cavanagh JF. Single-trial regression elucidates the role of prefrontal theta oscillations in response conflict. Front Psychol. 2011;2:30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Nigbur R, Cohen MX, Ridderinkhof KR, Sturmer B. Theta dynamics reveal domain-specific control over stimulus and response conflict. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24(5):1264–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cohen MX, Donner TH. Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110(12):2752–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Cavanagh JF, Zambrano-Vazquez L, Allen JJ. Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. Psychophysiology. 2012;49(2):220–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(8):414–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Van Veen V, Carter CS. The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(4):593–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Yeung N, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev. 2004;111(4):931–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Larson MJ, Clayson PE, Clawson A. Making sense of all the conflict: a theoretical review and critique of conflict-related ERPs. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;93(3):283–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Heidlmayr K, Kihlstedt M, Isel F. A review on the electroencephalography markers of stroop executive control processes. Brain Cogn. 2020;146:105637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Mansouri F, Shanbour A, Mazza F, Fettes P, Zariffa J, Downar J. Effect of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation and phase-locked transcranial pulsed current stimulation on learning and cognitive control. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:1181.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Veniero D, Strüber D, Thut G, Herrmann CS. Noninvasive brain stimulation techniques can modulate cognitive processing. Organizational Res Methods. 2019;22(1):116–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Levasseur-Moreau J, Fecteau S. Translational application of neuromodulation of decision-making. Brain Stimul. 2012;5(2):77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Pulopulos MM, Schmausser M, De Smet S, Vanderhasselt MA, Baliyan S, Venero C, Baeken C, De Raedt R. The effect of HF-rTMS over the left DLPFC on stress regulation as measured by cortisol and heart rate variability. Horm Behav. 2020;124.

  42. Thut G, Miniussi C. New insights into rhythmic brain activity from TMS-EEG studies. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(4):182–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Filmer HL, Dux PE, Mattingley JB. Applications of transcranial direct current stimulation for understanding brain function. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37(12):742–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Herrmann CS, Rach S, Neuling T, Struber D. Transcranial alternating current stimulation: a review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:279.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Brunoni AR, Vanderhasselt MA. Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. 2014;86:1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Brunye TT, Moran JM, Holmes A, Mahoney CR, Taylor HA. Non-invasive brain stimulation targeting the right fusiform gyrus selectively increases working memory for faces. Brain Cogn. 2017;113:32–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hill AT, Rogasch NC, Fitzgerald PB, Hoy KE. Impact of concurrent task performance on transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)-Induced changes in cortical physiology and working memory. Cortex. 2019;113:37–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Kim S-E, Kim H-S, Kwak Y, Ahn M-H, Choi KM, Min B-K. Neurodynamic correlates for the cross-frequency coupled transcranial alternating current stimulation during working memory performance. Front NeuroSci. 2022;16:1013691.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Schluter RS, Daams JG, van Holst RJ, Goudriaan AE. Effects of non-invasive neuromodulation on executive and other cognitive functions in addictive disorders: a systematic review. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:642.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Volpe G, Tagliente S, Palmisano A, Grattagliano I, Rivolta D. Non-invasive neuromodulation can reduce aggressive behaviors in humans: a critical perspective. J Forensic Sci. 2022;67(4):1593–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(10):474–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Johnson L, Alekseichuk I, Krieg J, Doyle A, Yu Y, Vitek J, Johnson M, Opitz A. Dose-dependent effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on spike timing in awake nonhuman primates. Sci Adv. 2020;6(36).

  53. Krause MR, Vieira PG, Csorba BA, Pilly PK, Pack CC. Transcranial alternating current stimulation entrains single-neuron activity in the primate brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(12):5747–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Gollo LL, Mirasso C, Sporns O, Breakspear M. Mechanisms of zero-lag synchronization in cortical motifs. PLoS Comput Biol. 2014;10(4):e1003548.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Doesburg SM, Green JJ, McDonald JJ, Ward LM. From local inhibition to long-range integration: a functional dissociation of alpha-band synchronization across cortical scales in visuospatial attention. Brain Res. 2009;1303:97–110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Palva S, Palva JM. Functional roles of alpha-band phase synchronization in local and large-scale cortical networks. Front Psychol. 2011;2:204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Reinhart RMG. Disruption and rescue of interareal theta phase coupling and adaptive behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114(43):11542–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Alekseichuk I, Pabel SC, Antal A, Paulus W. Intrahemispheric theta rhythm desynchronization impairs working memory. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2017;35(2):147–58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Tseng P, Iu KC, Juan CH. The critical role of phase difference in theta oscillation between bilateral parietal cortices for visuospatial working memory. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Polania R, Nitsche MA, Korman C, Batsikadze G, Paulus W. The importance of timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Curr Biol. 2012;22(14):1314–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Violante IR, Li LM, Carmichael DW, Lorenz R, Leech R, Hampshire A, Rothwell JC, Sharp DJ. Externally induced frontoparietal synchronization modulates network dynamics and enhances working memory performance. Elife. 2017;6.

  62. Thielscher A, Antunes A, Saturnino GB. Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: a useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS? In: 2015 37th annual international conference of the IEEE engineering in medicine and biology society (EMBC): 2015. IEEE: 222–225.

  63. De Boeck P, Jeon M. An Overview of models for response times and processes in cognitive tests. Front Psychol. 2019;10:102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134(1):9–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Makeig S, Debener S, Onton J, Delorme A. Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(5):204–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Pion-Tonachini L, Kreutz-Delgado K, Makeig S. ICLabel: an automated electroencephalographic independent component classifier, dataset, and website. NeuroImage. 2019;198:181–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Delpuech C, Permier J. Oscillatory gamma-band (30–70 hz) activity induced by a visual search task in humans. J Neurosci. 1997;17(2):722–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Min BK, Park HJ. Task-related modulation of anterior theta and posterior alpha EEG reflects top-down preparation. BMC Neurosci. 2010;11:79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev. 2007;53(1):63–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G* power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Pritchard WS. The brain in fractal time: 1/f-like power spectrum scaling of the human electroencephalogram. Int J Neurosci. 1992;66(1–2):119–29.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Hillyard SA, Teder-Salejarvi WA, Munte TF. Temporal dynamics of early perceptual processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1998;8(2):202–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Proverbio AM, Zani A. Time course of brain activation during graphemic/phonologic processing in reading: an ERP study. Brain Lang. 2003;87(3):412–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Petit JP, Midgley KJ, Holcomb PJ, Grainger J. On the time course of letter perception: a masked priming ERP investigation. Psychon Bull Rev. 2006;13(4):674–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Anllo-Vento L, Luck SJ, Hillyard SA. Spatio-temporal dynamics of attention to color: evidence from human electrophysiology. Hum Brain Mapp. 1998;6(4):216–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Taylor MJ. Non-spatial attentional effects on P1. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(12):1903–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Muller MM, Andersen S, Trujillo NJ, Valdes-Sosa P, Malinowski P, Hillyard SA. Feature-selective attention enhances color signals in early visual areas of the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(38):14250–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Zhang W, Luck SJ. Feature-based attention modulates feedforward visual processing. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(1):24–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Patel SH, Azzam PN. Characterization of N200 and P300: selected studies of the event-related potential. Int J Med Sci. 2005;2(4):147–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Folstein JR, Van Petten C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology. 2008;45(1):152–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Milham MP, Banich MT, Claus ED, Cohen NJ. Practice-related effects demonstrate complementary roles of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices in attentional control. NeuroImage. 2003;18(2):483–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Larson MJ, Clayson PE, Baldwin SA. Performance monitoring following conflict: internal adjustments in cognitive control? Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(3):426–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Millner AJ, Jaroszewski AC, Chamarthi H, Pizzagalli DA. Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of training-induced cognitive control improvements. NeuroImage. 2012;63(2):742–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. West R. Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the stroop and digit-location tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(8):1122–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Perlstein WM, Larson MJ, Dotson VM, Kelly KG. Temporal dissociation of components of cognitive control dysfunction in severe TBI: ERPs and the cued-Stroop task. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(2):260–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Cravo AM, Rohenkohl G, Wyart V, Nobre AC. Endogenous modulation of low frequency oscillations by temporal expectations. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(6):2964–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Cunillera T, Fuentemilla L, Perianez J, Marco-Pallares J, Kramer UM, Camara E, Munte TF, Rodriguez-Fornells A. Brain oscillatory activity associated with task switching and feedback processing. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012;12(1):16–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Braver TS. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(2):106–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. van Driel J, Swart JC, Egner T, Ridderinkhof KR, Cohen MX. (no) time for control: frontal theta dynamics reveal the cost of temporally guided conflict anticipation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2015;15(4):787–807.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Cooper PS, Karayanidis F, McKewen M, McLellan-Hall S, Wong ASW, Skippen P, Cavanagh JF. Frontal theta predicts specific cognitive control-induced behavioural changes beyond general reaction time slowing. NeuroImage. 2019;189:130–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Kaiser J, Iliopoulos P, Steinmassl K, Schutz-Bosbach S. Preparing for success: neural frontal theta and posterior alpha dynamics during action preparation predict flexible resolution of cognitive conflicts. J Cogn Neurosci. 2022;34(6):1070–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Wendiggensen P, Ghin F, Koyun AH, Stock AK, Beste C. Pretrial theta band activity affects context-dependent modulation of response inhibition. J Cogn Neurosci. 2022;34(4):605–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Min BK, Herrmann CS. Prestimulus EEG alpha activity reflects prestimulus top-down processing. Neurosci Lett. 2007;422(2):131–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Jensen O, Mazaheri A. Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:186.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Foxe JJ, Snyder AC. The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Front Psychol. 2011;2:154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Morrow A, Elias M, Samaha J. Evaluating the evidence for the functional inhibition account of alpha-band oscillations during Preparatory attention. J Cogn Neurosci. 2023;35(8):1195–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Luck SJ, Woodman GF, Vogel EK. Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(11):432–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Rohenkohl G, Nobre AC. Alpha oscillations related to anticipatory attention follow temporal expectations. J Neurosci. 2011;31(40):14076–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. Hwang K, Ghuman AS, Manoach DS, Jones SR, Luna B. Cortical neurodynamics of inhibitory control. J Neurosci. 2014;34(29):9551–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(3):99–105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  101. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:73–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Alekseichuk I, Wischnewski M, Opitz A. A minimum effective dose for (transcranial) alternating current stimulation. Brain Stimulation: Basic Translational Clin Res Neuromodulation. 2022;15(5):1221–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Reato D, Rahman A, Bikson M, Parra LC. Low-intensity electrical stimulation affects network dynamics by modulating population rate and spike timing. J Neurosci. 2010;30(45):15067–79.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Grossman N, Bono D, Dedic N, Kodandaramaiah SB, Rudenko A, Suk H-J, Cassara AM, Neufeld E, Kuster N, Tsai L-H. Noninvasive deep brain stimulation via temporally interfering electric fields. Cell. 2017;169(6):1029–41. e1016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Esmaeilpour Z, Kronberg G, Reato D, Parra LC, Bikson M. Temporal interference stimulation targets deep brain regions by modulating neural oscillations. Brain Stimul. 2021;14(1):55–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Kasten FH, Dowsett J, Herrmann CS. Sustained aftereffect of α-tACS lasts up to 70 min after stimulation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:245.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  107. Wischnewski M, Schutter DJ. After-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on evoked delta and theta power. Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;128(11):2227–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the Convergent Technology R&D Program for Human Augmentation (2020M3C1B8081319 to B.-K.M.) and Institute for Basic Science (IBS-R015-Y3 to H. K.) funded by the Korean government through the National Research Foundation of Korea.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

B.-K.M. conceived and designed the tACS neuromodulation paradigm using a phase lag of stimulation signals to investigate its neuromodulatory effects and stimulation-relevant changes in EEG signals of task-related regions, performed research. Y.K. and B.-K.M. wrote the main manuscript text. Y.K., J.-H.L., J.-C.P., J.K., H.K., J.S. and B.-K.M. performed the EEG experiment, analyzed data and reviewed the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Byoung-Kyong Min.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval and consent to participate

A study description was provided for all participants prior to the experiment. All participants signed the informed consent. This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Korea University Institutional Review Board (KUIRB-2021-0209-08). All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Consent for publication were given by all participants.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Supplementary Material 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, Y., Lee, JH., Park, JC. et al. Neuromodulation of inhibitory control using phase-lagged transcranial alternating current stimulation. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 21, 93 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-024-01385-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-024-01385-y

Keywords